Dive and beak movement patterns in leatherback turtles

Beak movements recorded in leatherbacks during the first hours of ..... movement events recorded in this study were actual biological events rather than artefacts. ...... male-male competition, and female tolerance through analysis of tooth-rake.
523KB taille 1 téléchargements 198 vues
Journal of Animal Ecology 2008, 77, 236–246

doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2008.01344.x

Dive and beak movement patterns in leatherback turtles Dermochelys coriacea during internesting intervals in French Guiana

Blackwell Publishing Ltd

Sabrina Fossette1*, Philippe Gaspar2, Yves Handrich1, Yvon Le Maho1 and Jean-Yves Georges1 1

IPHC-Département Ecologie, Physiologie, Ethologie, ULP, CNRS; 23 rue Becquerel, 67087 Strasbourg, France; and Collecte Localization Satellites, Space Oceanography Division, Marine Ecosystem Modelling and Monitoring by Satellites, 8–10 rue Hermès, 31520 Ramonville St Agne, France 2

Summary 1. Investigating the foraging patterns of free-ranging species is essential to estimate energy/time budgets for assessing their real reproductive strategy. Leatherback turtles Dermochelys coriacea (Vandelli 1761), commonly considered as capital breeders, have been reported recently to prospect actively during the breeding season in French Guiana, Atlantic Ocean. In this study we investigate the possibility of this active behaviour being associated with foraging, by studying concurrently diving and beak movement patterns in gravid females equipped with IMASEN (Inter-MAndibular Angle SENsor). 2. Four turtles provided data for periods varying from 7·3 to 56·1 h while exhibiting continuous short and shallow benthic dives. Beak movement (‘b-m’) events occurred in 34% of the dives, on average 1·8 ± 1·4 times per dive. These b-m events lasted between 1·5 and 20 s and occurred as isolated or grouped (two to five consecutive beak movements) events in 96·0 ± 4·0% of the recorded cases, and to a lesser extent in series (> five consecutive beak movements). 3. Most b-m events occurred during wiggles at the bottom of U- and W-shaped dives and at the beginning and end of the bottom phase of the dives. W-shaped dives were associated most frequently with beak movements (65% of such dives) and in particular with grouped beak movements. 4. Previous studies proposed wiggles to be indicator of predatory activity, U- and W-shaped dives being putative foraging dives. Beak movements recorded in leatherbacks during the first hours of their internesting interval in French Guiana may be related to feeding attempts. 5. In French Guiana, leatherbacks show different mouth-opening patterns for different dive patterns, suggesting that they forage opportunistically on occasional prey, with up to 17% of the dives appearing to be successful feeding dives. 6. This study highlights the contrasted strategies adopted by gravid leatherbacks nesting on the Pacific coasts of Costa Rica, in the deep-water Caribbean Sea and in the French Guianan shallow continental shelf, and may be related to different local prey accessibility among sites. Our results may help to explain recently reported site-specific individual body size and population dynamics. Key-words: alternative reproductive strategy, Atlantic Ocean, foraging, hall sensor, sea turtle

Introduction The resources (water, nutrients, energy) acquired by organisms in the wild are limited and cannot be allocated simultaneously to survival, growth and reproduction (Stearns 1992). Organisms are thus predicted to adopt strategies for facing trade-offs *Correspondence author: E-mail: [email protected]

between these competing life-history traits (Stearns 1992). For instance, organisms compensate for the high energy demands of reproduction by increasing the amount of total food resources available through two main strategies: schematically, capital breeders store body reserves prior to reproduction and rely exclusively on them during breeding; conversely, income breeders show limited storage capacities and adjust their food intake during reproduction (Jönsson 1997). However, in some cases where body reserves do not fulfil reproductive

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 British Ecological Society

Dive and beak movement patterns in leatherbacks 237 requirements, certain organisms considered commonly as capital breeders may refuel by feeding during the breeding season. Several examples of this intermediate strategy have been reported in birds (Durant, Massemin & Handrich 2004), ungulates (Byers & Moodie 1990), pinnipeds (Boness, Bowen & Oftedal 1994) and sea turtles (Hays et al. 2002; Myers & Hays 2006). Assessing real reproductive strategy by investigating the foraging patterns during and outside the breeding season is essential for the reliable estimation of energy and time budgets in order to establish ultimately how organisms ensure their reproduction and their own survival optimally (e.g. Boyd, Lunn & Barton 1991; Challet et al. 1994; Hochscheid et al. 1999; Georges & Guinet 2000). This has obvious consequences in terms of conservation in the case of species that feed on sites overlapping with human activities (e.g. Delord et al. 2005; Fossette et al. 2007b). Accordingly, there has been a general impetus to investigate in much greater detail the foraging behaviour of free-ranging species during their life cycle. In the case of marine species, where foraging occurs mainly underwater during dives, investigating foraging behaviour (i.e. diving pattern and actual feeding activity) is particularly difficult due to the limited accessibility of marine environments (Kooyman et al. 1992). Consequently, foraging behaviour in marine vertebrates has been explored in different ways based on two-dimensional time–depth dive profiles and threedimensional compass (e.g. Schreer & Testa 1996; Le Boeuf et al. 2000; Mitani et al. 2003), echolocation buzzes in sperm whales Physeter macrocephallus (e.g. Miller et al. 2004; Watwood et al. 2006), stomach/oesophageal/gastrointestinal temperature (e.g. Ancel, Horning & Kooyman 1997; Southwood et al. 2005) or underwater video (e.g. Reina et al. 2005; Watanabe et al. 2006). One of the most recent techniques is the recording of dive depth in conjunction with beak-opening events (e.g. Wilson et al. 2002; Wilson et al. 2003; Hochscheid et al. 2004; Myers & Hays 2006; Liebsch et al. 2007). This latest approach uses a magnet/Hall sensor combination connected to a data-logger that records changes in the intermandible angle with a high frequency [Inter-MAndibular Angle SENsor (IMASEN), see Wilson et al. 2002]. IMASENs have been proved to be a reliable system to investigate fine-scale ingestion and/or respiratory patterns in relation to diving behaviour. Given the relative difficulty of IMASEN deployment, this technique has been used mainly in captive animals (e.g. Ropert-Coudert et al. 2002, 2004; Wilson et al. 2002, 2003; Hochscheid et al. 2004; Liebsch et al. 2007). Data on free-living individuals concern only four different species of marine animals: Leptonychotes weddellii (the Weddell seal, n = 7 individuals, Plötz et al. 2001; Liebsch et al. 2007), Spheniscus magellanicus (the Magellanic penguin, n = 19 individuals, Wilson et al. 2002, 2003; Simeone & Wilson 2003), Pygoscelis antarctica (the Chinstrap penguin, n = 1 individual, Takahashi et al. 2004) and Dermochelys coriacea (the leatherback turtle, n = 1 individual, Myers & Hays 2006). In sea turtles, reproduction consists of successive ovipositions on land separated by internesting intervals of a few days, during which turtles remain at sea for egg maturation. Sea

turtles are considered commonly to be capital breeders, i.e. they ensure reproduction by relying on body reserves stored prior to reproduction during migration (Miller 1997). However, previous studies suggest that leatherbacks nesting in the Atlantic Ocean may forage during the nesting season (Eckert et al. 1989; Hays et al. 2004; Fossette et al. 2007b; Georges et al. 2007). This suggestion has been supported by Myers & Hays (2006), who reported some rhythmic beak movements in a single leatherback equipped with an IMASEN during the nesting season on Grenada Island (Atlantic Ocean). These rhythmic beak movements occurred as the animal was descending to depth and have been interpreted as potential attempts to detect prey by relying on gustatory cues to sense the surrounding waters (Myers & Hays 2006). Such putative foraging behaviour in Atlantic leatherbacks may help to compensate for particularly high reproductive costs. However, in the Pacific Ocean leatherbacks nesting in Costa Rica appear to compensate for high reproductive costs (Wallace et al. 2007) by reducing their activity (Reina et al. 2005) and their metabolic rate (Wallace et al. 2005) during internesting intervals. These two different strategies may be linked with different reproductive outputs, i.e. fewer nests and fewer eggs in Pacific populations (Wallace et al. 2006) and/or with different oceanographic conditions and local food availability (Hays et al. 2002; Saba et al. 2007). The Atlantic Ocean hosts the world’s two largest leatherback nesting populations, found within the large estuary of the Maroni river at the border between French Guiana and Suriname (South America) and on the coasts of Gabon (western Africa, review in Fossette et al. in press). In the Guianas region, the leatherback’s nesting season coincides with the peak of Amazonian influence where rich and turbid water masses cross off the continental shelf, enhancing biological production (Froidefond et al. 2002; Baklouti et al. 2007; S. Fossette, T. Bastian, F. Blanchard, C. Girard, Y. Le Maho, P. Vendeville & J.-Y. Georges, unpublished data). This is illustrated by massive stranding of jellyfish on the leatherback rookery of Awala-Yalimapo beach (French Guiana) and by the presence of large amounts of gelatinous plankton near the seabed in shallow waters during the leatherback nesting season (S. Fossette, T. Bastian, F. Blanchard, C. Girard, Y. Le Maho, P. Vendeville & J.-Y. Georges, unpublished data). As leatherbacks feed primarily on jellyfish (James & Herman 2001), this suggests favourable local trophic conditions for gravid females feeding during the nesting season in French Guiana. Consistently, leatherbacks have been reported to disperse widely across the Guiana continental shelf (Georges et al. 2007) and to dive continuously day and night, concentrating mainly on the area close to the seabed where they perform numerous vertical excursions of several metres of amplitude at the bottom of the dives (hereafter called ‘wiggles’; Fossette et al. 2007b). Wiggles are interpreted commonly as prospecting and foraging behaviour (e.g. Schreer, Kovacs & O’Hara Hines 2001; Simeone & Wilson 2003), so it has been suggested that leatherbacks may attempt to feed on the Guiana continental shelf (Fossette et al. 2007b). This hypothesis is supported further by the fact that leatherbacks

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 British Ecological Society, Journal of Animal Ecology, 77, 236–246

238

S. Fossette et al.

nesting in French Guiana are larger and heavier compared to those present on other nesting sites, suggesting either that they forage during the nesting season and/or that there is a sitespecific growth strategy in this species (Georges & Fossette 2006). However, there is no direct evidence to date of leatherbacks actually feeding in French Guiana. Here, we test the hypothesis that in French Guiana gravid leatherbacks do forage during the nesting season by jointly investigating the fine-scale diving and beak movement patterns. We predict that if leatherbacks in French Guiana attempt to forage during the nesting season, mouth-openings should occur at least during the descent phase of dives for detecting prey (as reported by Myers & Hays 2006), but also during wiggles performed at the bottom of benthic dives (as suggested by Fossette et al. 2007b).

Materials and methods Fieldwork was conducted during the leatherback’s nesting season in May–June 2006 at Awala-Yalimapo beach (5·7° N–53·9° W), French Guiana, South America.

INSTRUMENT DEPLOYMENT

Twelve turtles were selected opportunistically during our night patrolling on the beach and equipped with an IMASEN (Terralog, JUV Elektronic; Borstel, Germany). The IMASEN consisted of a three-channel data logger (70 × 50 × 20 mm, 55 g in air, data resolution: 16 bit, memory size: 128 Mbit), sampling at the same frequency (8 Hz for six turtles and 4 Hz for six other turtles) depth (range: 0–200 m) and strength of the magnetic field from which jaw movements can be extrapolated. The logger was cast in resin and powered by a lithium 3·6-V battery (Sonnenschein Lithium GmbH, Büdingen Germany). Theoretically, these sampling protocols allow 92 and 184 h of recording for 8 and 4 Hz sampling frequency, respectively, the total amount of data stored being limited by instrument memory capacity rather than by the battery. The devices were attached at night during oviposition which took approximately 10 min. The logger was fixed on the pseudo-carapace using 4·5 mm wide plastic nylon ties introduced in 6 mm diameter holes drilled in a lateral dorsal ridge (similar to Fossette et al. 2007a). The logger was connected with a 1 m cable to a Hall sensor (15 × 5 × 3 mm), highly sensitive to magnetic field, set in epoxy and glued onto the upper jaw directly onto the skin (see Myers & Hays 2006). A small, rare earth magnet (10 × 10 × 2 mm) also set in epoxy was glued onto the lower jaw face to the Hall sensor, directly onto the skin using cyanoacrylite glue (see Myers & Hays 2006). Dimensions of the Hall sensor and the magnet set in epoxy were approximately 30 × 15 × 3 mm, necessitating a minimum distance of 2·5 cm between sensor and magnet locations. IMASENs are based on the Hall effect, where an increase in the distance between the Hall sensor and the magnet (as the beak opened) results in a decrease in the magnetic field strength, recorded by the logger as a drop in voltage. The Hall sensor output can thus be related theoretically to the intermandible distance by calibrating the logger before deployment once fixed on the turtle. For each logger, an exponential relationship was obtained between the magnetic field strength and the intermandible distance, with differences noted among loggers due to different sensitivity and position of the sensor relative to the magnet. However, for all loggers, the magnetic field

Fig. 1. Example of a record of pressure and Hall signal obtained in a leatherback turtle concurrently equipped with a time depth recorder and an Inter-MAndibular Angle SENsor (IMASEN) during one internesting interval in French Guiana in 2006. Note that the raw Hall signal (bottom) was highly affected by pressure (middle), which implied to filter Hall data (top). Consequently, beak movement patterns were investigated in terms of occurrence rather than quantitatively (see Methods).

strength did not vary for intermandible distances larger than 3·5 cm. According to the narrow reading window (2·5 –3·5 cm), most (> 95%) of the recorded data could not be used for quantitatively estimating mouth-opening amplitude. In addition, the baseline of the Hall signal was affected highly by pressure and changed parallel to the diving profile (Fig. 1). Simple mathematical high-pass filtering (butterhigh and filtfilt functions in  version 6, > 6 s) was used to smooth the voltage baseline (Fig. 1). However, due to the abovementioned patterns, we did not investigate the amplitude change patterns of the Hall signal. Consequently, this study is based on the pattern of beak-opening event occurrence.

DATA ANALYSIS

IMASENs data were analysed using MT Dive software (Jensen Software Systems 2006, Laboe, Germany). Individual dives were defined as any depth exceeding 3 m. For each dive, the program calculated the start and end time, the maximum depth reached during the dive, dive duration, the post-dive surface interval, time spent at the bottom of the dive, the number of wiggles and the number of dive steps. The bottom phase was defined as the period during which (a) depth was greater than 70% of maximum depth of a given dive and (b) the vertical speed was less than 0·3 ms–1. Wiggles were defined for any event involving a rapid opposite change in depth > 1·2 m (equivalent to c. twice the maximum ventro-dorsal height of a leatherback turtle; Georges & Fossette 2006) during the bottom phase of the dive. ‘Dive steps’ were defined as any events where vertical rate was less than 0·3 ms–1 during the descent or ascent phase of the dive (Hochscheid et al. 1999). Dives were then classified into three types according to their profiles (see Fossette et al. 2007b) – ‘V-type’ and ‘U-type’ dives – where bottom phase duration was < 30% and > 30% of dive duration, respectively, and ‘W-type’ (U-type dive showing at least one wiggle with an amplitude of 40% of the maximum wiggle amplitude recorded for a given turtle).

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 British Ecological Society, Journal of Animal Ecology, 77, 236–246

Dive and beak movement patterns in leatherbacks 239

Results Among the 12 turtles equipped with IMASEN, nine individuals returned to the nesting beach. All had lost their magnet and/ or Hall sensor and two had also lost their logger. Of the seven returned loggers, three had no recorded data. Consequently, only four turtles provided data for recorded periods varying from 7·3 h (no. 4) to 56·1 h (no. 2) (Table 1).

OVERALL DIVING BEHAVIOUR

The 1009 dives recorded from the four turtles were shallow (11·8 ± 6·3 m) and short (6·6 ± 2·6 min, Table 1), with 50% and 75% of overall dives shallower than 10·7 m and 16·7 m and shorter than 6·2 min and 8·2 min, respectively (Fig. 3). Within a dive, the time spent at the bottom lasted on average 2·6 ± 2·1 min (i.e. 38·9 ± 23·2% of the total dive duration). Wiggles occurred in 65·3% of the dives, with a mean of 2·4 ± 1·0 wiggles per dive. The mean hourly diving effort was 46·0 ± 12·1 min spent diving per hour. V-, U- and W-shaped dives represented 37·7 ± 15·4%, 54·3 ± 17·3%, and 8·0 ± 4·0% of overall dives, respectively (mean ± SD of the four turtles, Table 2). U-shaped dives were shallower than V- and W-shaped dives (Table 2). W-shaped dives were longer than U- and V-shaped dives (Table 2) due to their longer bottom time.

DIVES ASSOCIATED WITH BEAK MOVEMENT EVENTS

Fig. 2. Example of Hall signals and dive profiles for two leatherbacks during their internesting interval in French Guiana in 2006; (a) turtle no. 1; (b) turtle no. 2. Note that most beak movement events occurred at the surface (breath) but beak movement events eventually occurred underwater either as isolated events (d, h), grouped events (b, c) or in series (a, f ) during either the descent, bottom or ascent phase of the dives. (c) Enlarged profiles of isolated, grouped and series of beak movements (from left to right).

Filtered IMASEN sensor output was used to detect beak movements (Figs 1 and 2). A preliminary visual inspection of the filtered Hall and diving data permitted the distinction of beak movements occurring at the surface from those occurring during dives. Their similar shape and amplitude indicated that underwater beak movement events recorded in this study were actual biological events rather than artefacts. For each dive, we then identified beak movements visually, defined as any drop in voltage with at least twice the amplitude of the background noise. For each dive, we recorded manually the number of beak movement events, the dive phase (descent, bottom or ascent phase) during which they occurred, and also noted if they were associated or not with either wiggles or dive steps. Statistical analyses were carried out using Minitab® statistical software. Values in percentage were arcsin-transformed. Values are given as means ± standard deviation (SD).

Among the 1009 recorded dives, 342 dives were associated with at least one beak movement event (i.e. 32·7 ± 5·0%, range: 26·2–36·8%, Fig. 2). Those dives associated with beak movements (hereafter called ‘b-m’ dives) were mainly shallow (14·0 ± 6·6 m) and short (7·5 ± 2·8 min), but were also deeper (t-test, t342,667 = 7·88, P < 0·001) and longer (t342,667 = 7·77, P < 0·001) than the remaining 667 non-b-m dives (dive depth: 10·7 ± 5·9 m; dive duration: 6·1 ± 2·4 min, Fig. 3). Among the 342 b-m dives, 50% and 75% were shallower than 13·4 and 19·4 m, and shorter than 7·4 min and 9·5 min, respectively (Fig. 3). The turtles performed between 2·3 and 2·6 b-m dives per hour (Table 1). The b-m dives were isolated throughout time rather than distributed in successive bouts (sensu Gentry & Kooyman 1986). The proportion of b-m dives among overall dives increased significantly with dive depth and dive duration when either considering each turtle individually (Spearman’s rank correlation, P < 0·05 in all cases) or when considering all turtles together (Spearman’s rank correlation for the grand mean of dive depth: RS = 0·913, n = 4 turtles, P = 0·011, Fig. 4a, grand mean of dive duration: RS = 0·866, n = 4 turtles, P < 0·001, Fig. 4b). Regarding dive shapes, b-m dives were mainly U-shaped and V-shaped (44·7 ± 13·9% and 42·2 ± 10·9%, respectively, for the four turtles). W-shaped dives represented only 13·1 ± 3·8% of the b-m dives. However, among all W-shaped dives, 64·7 ± 25·4% were associated with beak movements (i.e. 4·6% of the 1009 recorded dives) compared to 27·8 ± 8·3% of all

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 British Ecological Society, Journal of Animal Ecology, 77, 236–246

240

S. Fossette et al.

Table 1. Summary of diving and beak movement (b-m) occurrence in four leatherback turtles during one internesting interval in French Guiana in 2006. Values are expressed as mean ± standard deviation (SD) Turtles ID no.

1

2

3

4

All turtles mean ± SD

Departure time Recorded period (h) Total no. of dives recorded Mean dive depth (m) Mean dive duration (min) Mean hourly diving effort (min/h) Total no. of b-m dives Mean no. of b-m dives/h Mean depth of b-m dives (m) Mean duration of b-m dives (min) Total no. of b-m events Mean no. of b-m events per b-m dives % of isolated b-m events % of groups of b-m events % of series of b-m events

23/05/2006 02 : 30 35·3 253 16·2 ± 6·2 6·3 ± 2·5 44·1 ± 11·1 92 2·6 ± 1·9 18·1 ± 6·3 6·9 ± 2·9 124 1·4 ± 0·7

7/06/2006 23 : 26 56·1 407 8·8 ± 3·9 6·1 ± 2·2 47·2 ± 10·2 128 2·3 ± 1·8 10·4 ± 3·7 7·3 ± 2·5 232 1·8 ± 1·4

9/06/2006 05 : 28 46·3 288 13·7 ± 6·3 8·1 ± 2·8 50·5 ± 6·6 106 2·3 ± 1·5 16·2 ± 6·4 8·8 ± 2·7 229 2·2 ± 1·9

26/06/2006 22 : 50 7·3 61 4·8 ± 1·7 4·3 ± 1·7 37·4 ± 16·2 16 2·3 ± 1·5 4·9 ± 1·9 4·4 ± 2·0 20 1·3 ± 0·4

1009 11·8 ± 6·3 6·6 ± 2·6 46·0 ± 12·1 342 2·4 ± 1·7 14·0 ± 6·6 7·5 ± 2·8 605 1·8 ± 1·4

69·4 29·0 1·6

41·4 50·9 7·7

49·3 44·1 6·6

60·0 40·0 0

55·0 ± 12·2 41·0 ± 9·1 4·0 ± 3·8

Table 2. Summary of dive parameters and beak movement (b-m) occurrence in relation to dive shape (U, V and W) in four leatherback turtles during one internesting interval in French Guiana in 2006. Differences in means and proportions were tested statistically using χ2, analysis of variance () followed by a post-hoc Tukey test or Kruskal–Wallis test followed by a post-hoc Bonferroni test. Different letters (a, b) indicate significant (P < 0·05) differences among groups. Values are expressed as mean ± standard deviation Dive type

All dives

U-shaped

V-shaped

W-shaped

Test, P-value

No. Frequency (%) Dive depth (m) Dive duration (min) % with b-m events No. of b-m events per b-m dives

1009 – 11·8 ± 6·3 6·6 ± 2·6 32·7 ± 5·0 1·8 ± 1·4

475 54·3 ± 17·3 11·1 ± 6·6a 6·5 ± 2·5a 27·8 ± 8·3a 1·9 ± 0·5a

444 37·7 ± 15·4 12·5 ± 5·9b 6·3 ± 2·3a 38·2 ± 7·8a, b 1·5 ± 0·5a

90 8·0 ± 4·0 13·1 ± 5·7b 9·4 ± 2·8b 64·7 ± 25·4b 1·6 ± 0·4a

– χ2, χ22 = 180·8, P < 0·001 , F2,1007 = 7·53, P < 0·01 , F2,1007 = 60·1, P < 0·001 Kruskal–Wallis, H2,12 = 7·54, P < 0·05 Kruskal–Wallis, H2,12 = 1·76, P = 0·415

U-shaped dives (i.e. 14·1% of the 1009 recorded dives) and 38·3 ± 7·8% of all V-shaped dives (i.e. 15·2% of the 1009 recorded dives, Table 2).

OCCURRENCE OF BEAK MOVEMENT EVENTS DURING THE DIVE

Beak movements were recorded both at the surface and during dives. In both situations, three categories of beak movement events were defined: (a) isolated beak movements (lasting between 1·5 and 4 s); (b) group of two to five successive beak movements (lasting between 3 and 20 s in total); and (c) series of more than five successive beak movements (lasting between 10 s and 5 min in total; Fig. 2). Less than 1% of the events recorded at the surface were isolated beak movements. The category of a beak movement event recorded during a surface time was not linked to the occurrence or the category of beak movement events in the previous or following dive. Among the b-m dives, beak movement events occurred on average 1·8 ± 1·4 times per dive (range: 1–11, Tables 1 and Fig. 2). The mean number of beak movement events per dive was not significantly different on comparison of the three types of dive profiles (Table 2). Beak movement occurrence

was mainly isolated (55·0 ± 12·2% of the events) and grouped (41·0 ± 9·1% of the events), rather than in series (4·0 ± 3·8% of the events; Tables 1 and Fig. 2). Thus, 96·0 ± 4·0% of beak movement events during a dive lasted between 1·5 and 20 s. There was one single isolated and one single grouped beak movement event in 36·8 ± 9·7%, and in 25·5 ± 8·4% of the b-m dives, respectively. There were several isolated and several grouped beak movements in 12·6 ± 4·5%, and in 11·6 ± 4·9% of the b-m dives, respectively. In 5·1 ± 3·9% of the b-m dives, isolated beak movements preceded groups of beak movements, while it was the contrary for 3·0 ± 2·6% of the b-m dives. This pattern was similar when considering U-shaped and V-shaped b-m dives separately. However, for W-shaped b-m dives, a single group of beak movements occurred in 50·3 ± 33·8% of the cases, and groups of beak movements preceded isolated beak movements in 10·4 ± 10·8%. In addition, W-shaped dives presented significantly more grouped beak movement events than other types of beak movement events, whereas U-shaped and V-shaped dives presented significantly more isolated and grouped beak movement events than series of events (Table 3). No significant increase was seen in the mean number of beak movement events per b-m dive with dive depth or dive

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 British Ecological Society, Journal of Animal Ecology, 77, 236–246

Dive and beak movement patterns in leatherbacks 241

Fig. 3. Frequency distribution of (a) dive depth and (b) dive duration for all recorded dives (n = 1009 dives, in black), dives without beak movement events (n = 667 non-b-m dives, in grey) and dives associated with beak movement (n = 342 b-m dives, in white) in four leatherback turtles during one internesting interval in French Guiana in 2006.

duration, when considering either each turtle individually [analysis of variance (), P > 0·05 in all cases except turtle no. 2, which showed an increase with dive depth, F3,124 = 3·14, n = 127 dives, P = 0·03] or all turtles together (Kruskal–Wallis, dive depth: H5,18 = 4·14, n = 4 turtles, P = 0·53; dive duration: H13,43 = 18·3, n = 4 turtles, P = 0·14). During U-shaped and W-shaped b-m dives, beak movement events occurred mainly during the bottom phase (71·6 ± 12·6% and 84·8 ± 20·3%, respectively, Table 3, Figs 2 and 5) whatever their category (isolated, group or series). During V-shaped dives, beak movements occurred similarly during the descent (29·6 ± 17·6%), the bottom (55·8 ± 23·4%) or the ascent

Fig. 4. Proportion of dives associated with beak movement events among all recorded dives in relation to (a) dive depth and (b) dive duration in four leatherback turtles during their internesting interval in French Guiana in 2006. Individual relations were calculated with all recorded dives for each turtle (turtles 1, 2, 3 and 4 in open circles, open squares, open triangles and open diamonds, respectively) and for pooled data (mean ± standard deviation, bold open circles, n = 4 individuals).

phase of the dive (14·7 ± 8·0%, Table 3, Figs 2 and 5) except for isolated beak movements, which occurred mainly during both the descent and the bottom phase of the V-shaped dives (38·8 ± 16·6% and 44·4 ± 22·8%, respectively). There was no significant relationship between the proportion of time spent at the bottom of the dive and the number of beak movement events per dive, whatever their category (Pearson’s correlation, P > 0·05). The majority of beak movement events (62·7%) corresponded to a change in swimming direction, i.e. either a wiggle during the bottom phase (in 28·0 ± 8·0% of the recorded cases, n = 4 turtles), or the

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 British Ecological Society, Journal of Animal Ecology, 77, 236–246

242

S. Fossette et al.

Table 3. Proportions of beak movement (b-m) occurrence and types (isolated, grouped, in series) in relation to dive shape (U, V and W) in four leatherback turtles during one internesting interval in French Guiana in 2006. Differences in means and proportions were tested statistically using a Kruskal–Wallis test followed by a post-hoc Bonferroni test (n = 4 turtles). Different letters (a, b) indicate significant (P < 0·05) differences among groups

Dive type

% of b-m events in descent phase

% of b-m events in bottom phase

% of b-m events in ascent phase

Test P-value

% isolated b-m events

% grouped b-m events

% series of b-m events

Test P-value

U-shaped

13·7 ± 13·0a

71·6 ± 12·6b

14·7 ± 7·8a

57·3 ± 18·4a

38·7 ± 15·2a

4·0 ± 3·3b

V-shaped

29·6 ± 17·6a

55·8 ± 23·4a

14·7 ± 8·0a

58·9 ± 15·7a

37·1 ± 13·8a

4·0 ± 4·6b

W-shaped

8·7 ± 13·8a

84·8 ± 20·3b

6·5 ± 7·0a

H2,12 = 7·4, P < 0·05 H2,12 = 5·5, P = 0·06 H2,12 = 7·4, P < 0·05

24·9 ± 18·7a

72·1 ± 21·2b

3·0 ± 3·5a

H2,12 = 8·0, P < 0·05 H2,12 = 9·0, P < 0·05 H2,12 = 8·3, P < 0·05

Fig. 5. Frequency of beak movement events recorded during each phase of the dive (descent, bottom and ascent) for the three types of dives (U, V and W) associated with beak movements (b-m dives) in four leatherback turtles during one internesting interval in French Guiana in 2006. Kruskal–Wallis followed by a post-hoc Bonferroni test (*P < 0·05; NS: P > 0·05).

beginning (11·9 ± 8·7%) and the end (7·9 ± 5·1%) of the bottom phase, or the peak of the V-shaped dives (11·4 ± 1·4%), or a ‘dive step’ (3·5 ± 5·1%) (Fig. 2). The percentage of beak movement events associated with a wiggle was not significantly different among the three categories of beak movement events (H2,11 = 3·3, n = 4 turtles, P = 0·19). On average, 39·5 ± 21·4% and 36·1 ± 12·3% of groups and series of beak movements, respectively, were associated with a wiggle and 23·9 ± 2·5% of isolated beak movements.

Discussion There is considerable interest in assessing the foraging behaviour of marine vertebrates and particularly in establishing the role of different dive types (e.g. Lesage, Hammil & Kovacs

1999; Schreer et al. 2001), quantifying foraging effort (e.g. Ropert-Coudert et al. 2002; Watwood et al. 2006) and foraging success (e.g. Wilson et al. 2002; Austin et al. 2006) for estimating energy and time budgets of these predators and their trophic role in their ecosystems. Several attempts have been made within this context to study the case of sea turtles (e.g. Hochscheid et al. 1999, 2004; Myers & Hays 2006). The present work aims to contribute to this impetus and constitutes the first study where several free-ranging IMASEN-equipped sea turtles are monitored to assess the potential links between diving and mouth-opening patterns. Among the 12 turtles equipped initially in this study, three were not resighted later in the nesting season. As patrolling occurred every night throughout the study period, this suggests that these turtles initiated their post-nesting migration after deployment. All other turtles were observed renesting normally (S. F. and J. Y. G., personal observation) 7·0 –10·9 days after, as reported previously in the area (Georges et al. 2007), and did not show any signs of injuries either on the carapace where the loggers were fixed or on the beak where the sensors and the magnets were glued. This suggests that our magnetic devices did not have any welfare or behavioural impacts on the study animals, as reported for other species (Ropert-Coudert et al. 2004; Takahashi et al. 2004). Consistently, Luschi et al. (2007) report that magnets producing fields stronger than Earth’s magnetic field may impair sea turtles’ navigational performances without, however, preventing the turtles from eventually reaching their goals. Accordingly, the beakopening events observed in the present study were considered as normal activity rather than as artefacts associated with animal disturbance.

GENERAL DIVING PATTERN

In our study, four leatherbacks were monitored successfully during a maximum of 2 days. In French Guiana, these first days correspond to the period of the internesting interval, when leatherbacks remain in shallow (< 50 m deep) coastal (within 50 km from the coasts) waters before dispersing extensively over the continental shelf (Fossette et al. 2007b; Georges et al. 2007). Consistently, the four leatherbacks exhibited continuous short and shallow putative benthic

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 British Ecological Society, Journal of Animal Ecology, 77, 236–246

Dive and beak movement patterns in leatherbacks 243 dives and dived for more than 45 min per hour, as reported previously in the area (Fossette et al. 2007b) and in other sites, both in the Atlantic and in the Pacific oceans (e.g. Eckert et al. 1996; Southwood et al. 1999). Our four study leatherbacks performed mainly U- or W-shaped dives (60% of all dives), spending about 30% of their time at the bottom of the dive where they performed numerous wiggles. U- and W-shaped dives are commonly considered as foraging dives during which animals spend time in a food patch, but they may also be associated with resting, as reported in the green turtle Chelonia mydas (Hochscheid et al. 1999). Conversely, V-shaped dives are considered as travelling or exploration (e.g. Hindell, Slip & Burton 1991; Le Boeuf et al. 1992, 2000; Schreer & Testa 1996; Lesage et al. 1999; Hays et al. 2002). The continuous benthic diving associated with extended movements over the continental shelf indicate that in French Guiana gravid leatherbacks are active rather than quiescent, contrary to the behaviour reported in gravid leatherbacks on the Pacific coasts of Costa Rica (Reina et al. 2005).

GENERAL BEAK MOVEMENT PATTERN

In our study all monitored leatherbacks showed beak-opening activity, both at the surface and during dives, during the first hours of their internesting interval. One-third of the dives were associated with beak movements that occurred irregularly during dives rather than rhythmically or in long series through time. In marine vertebrates, mouth-opening activity may be involved in several kinds of behaviour: communication (e.g. Wilson et al. 2002), courtship (e.g. Booth & Peters 1972; Reina et al. 2005), drinking (e.g. Lutz 1997; Myers & Hays 2006), foraging (e.g. Wilson et al. 2002; Hochscheid et al. 2004; Takahashi et al. 2004; Myers & Hays 2006; Liebsch et al. 2007) and breathing when beak movements occur at the surface (e.g. Wilson et al. 2003; Reina et al. 2005). For communication and courtship, social and sexual interactions are initiated commonly by adult males in marine vertebrates (reviewed in Miller 1997 and Scott et al. 2005). Females are not generally observed acting aggressively towards adults of either sex (e.g. bottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus, Scott et al. 2005; leatherbacks, Reina et al. 2005), with the exception of Caretta caretta (the loggerhead sea turtle), where female–female aggressive interactions may occur eventually, yet in low frequency, on resting sites rather than during basking or swimming (Schofield et al. 2007). In French Guiana, where leatherback turtles clearly do not rest during their internesting intervals, the relatively high frequency of beak movement events reported in the present study suggests that they may not be associated with sexual or social interactions. Regarding drinking, adult sea turtles have been reported to normally avoid drinking sea water directly (Lutz 1997; Southwood et al. 2005). This suggests that beak-opening events observed in our study may not be associated with drinking. However, if beak movements were associated with drinking, their pattern should have been similar to that of leatherbacks from different areas. The irregular pattern of beak movements observed in our four leatherbacks in French

Guiana contrasts, however, with the continuous rhythmic beak movements reported in an IMASEN-equipped leatherback performing an internesting interval in the Caribbean Sea (Myers & Hays 2006). The beak movement patterns recorded in our study may not, therefore, be associated exclusively with drinking. The last function of beak movement events may be associated with foraging. In our study, beak movement events occurred irregularly, primarily (62·3 ± 18·0%) as one isolated beak movement or one group of beak movements during the dive, and secondly (24·3 ± 9·4%) as successive isolated beak movements or groups of beak movements (see Fig. 2). Similar irregular patterns of mouth-opening events have been reported in several species of marine vertebrates, where the number, duration, frequency and amplitude of mouth movements depends on whether the predator attempts then succeeds or not to catch its prey, then manipulates, swallows or ingests it (Wilson et al. 2002; Hochscheid et al. 2004; Ropert-Coudert et al. 2004; Liebsch et al. 2007). In our study, beak-opening events lasted mainly between 1·5 and 20 s. This is similar to results obtained in captive loggerhead turtles feeding on crabs, where isolated beak movements last 1–5 s and a complete feeding cycle lasts from 10 to 50 s until prey ingestion, depending on prey type (Hochscheid et al. 2004). Similar durations of feeding events have been reported in free-living penguins (0·1–11 s; Wilson et al. 2002; Takahashi et al. 2004) and in free-ranging Weddell seals (0·6–30·7 s; Liebsch et al. 2007), the interspecies differences in feeding duration probably being related to differences in prey size among species. In all cases, captured prey is transported intra-orally thanks to repeated mouth-opening and -closing events. Consistently, the groups and series of successive beak movements observed in our study may correspond to successful feeding events and food processing. Conversely, isolated beak movements may be interpreted as unsuccessful capture attempts. Nevertheless, unsuccessful attacks are considered unlikely in our study, as leatherbacks feed mainly on slowly moving gelatinous plankton. Because isolated beak movements represent 55% of the beak movement events recorded in our study and mainly precede putative foraging grouped beak movements, isolated beak movements may, rather, permit the turtle to sense the surrounding waters for detecting potential prey olfactory signatures, as suggested by Myers & Hays (2006). In our study, beak movements were also recorded at the surface. They were mainly in groups and series and were interpreted classically as breathing events. However, if an animal feeds at the surface this might be difficult to separate from the breathing signal. Leatherbacks have been observed feeding on large jellyfish at the surface (James & Herman 2001). Such observations have never been conducted in French Guiana, probably because of the smaller size of jellyfish (unpublished data). In our study, there was no correlation between the type of beak-opening events at the surface and the occurrence of beak movements during the preceding/ following dive. This suggests that surface beak movements reported in leatherbacks are probably not associated with food processing. Nevertheless, feeding on subsurface prey

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 British Ecological Society, Journal of Animal Ecology, 77, 236–246

244

S. Fossette et al.

cannot be excluded, even if this phenomenon has never been observed in French Guiana.

DIVE TYPE AND FORAGING EVENTS

The combined analysis of dive types and mouth-opening patterns provides an additional means of determining underwater activity in free-ranging animals (e.g. Plötz et al. 2001; Liebsch et al. 2007). In our study, most of the beak movement events, whatever their category, occurred during the bottom phase of U- and W-shaped dives. In penguins and elephant seals (Mirounga longirostris), beak-opening events associated with prey ingestion have been reported to occur mainly during the bottom phase of U- and W-shaped dives, rather than during the descending and ascending phases (Le Boeuf et al. 1992; Wilson et al. 2002; Takahashi et al. 2004), suggesting that animals swim directly to and from a depth where food is located. This suggests that in our study, beak-opening events occurring during U- and W-shaped dives may correspond to foraging events, as suggested by Fossette et al. (2007b). Additionally, the present study proposes the idea that, in French Guiana, leatherbacks may attempt to feed during the entire bottom phase of the dives, probably as long as they are close to the seabed where their prey is concentrated (Artigas et al. 2003; S. Fossette, T. Bastian, F. Blanchard, C. Girard, Y. Le Maho, P. Vendeville & J.-Y. Georges, unpublished data). Indeed, dives associated with beak movements were slightly but significantly deeper and longer than other dives, and their proportion increased as turtles dived deeper and for longer. This suggests that in French Guiana, the probability for a leatherback to find prey increases as it dives for a longer time at deeper depth, at least during the first 2 days of the internesting interval. In French Guiana, beak movement events, whatever their category, occurred mainly when turtles were changing their vertical angle, either during wiggles or at the beginning and the end of the bottom phase. Additionally, W-shaped dives (i.e. dives with one or more ample wiggles), although less frequent than other dive types, were those associated most frequently with beak movements (65% of such dives) and in particular with grouped beak movements. Previous studies have shown that marine vertebrates capture prey mainly during wiggles at the bottom phase (Schreer et al. 2001; Simeone & Wilson 2003; Takahashi et al. 2004) and that undulations in the depth profile over time are associated with prey pursuit (Simeone & Wilson 2003; Takahashi et al. 2004). If this holds for leatherbacks, this suggests that the groups and series of beak movements associated with wiggles reported in our study may be potential indicators of foraging success. This supports our recent hypothesis that W-shaped dives have a foraging function in leatherback turtles (Fossette et al. 2007b). Conversely, V-shaped dives were associated mainly with isolated beak movements during the descent and the bottom phases. In marine vertebrates, V-shaped dives are interpreted commonly as exploration dives, a significant portion of the descent being devoted to prey searching (e.g. Hindell et al. 1991; Le Boeuf et al. 1992, 2000; Schreer & Testa 1996; Myers

& Hays 2006; Watwood et al. 2006). If this theory is also applicable to leatherbacks, this would support the hypothesis that leatherbacks taste the surrounding waters for detecting potential prey as they descend to depths (see Myers & Hays 2006), rather than actually feeding.

Conclusion Our study shows that in French Guiana, gravid leatherback turtles perform different patterns of mouth-opening associated with different dive patterns within the first days of their internesting intervals (this study), during which they disperse extensively over the continental shelf where they then perform benthic dives continuously (Fossette et al. 2007b; Georges et al. 2007). This indicates that, in French Guiana, gravid leatherbacks do indeed feed, or at least attempt to do so, during the nesting season. Dives associated with beak movement events were isolated throughout time, rather than distributed in successive dives inside a bout (sensu Gentry & Kooyman 1986), suggesting that leatherbacks forage opportunistically on dispersed prey, with up to 17% of the dives appearing to be successful feeding dives. The actual function of the different beak-opening patterns is, however, more difficult to identify, and further validation is required either by in-captivity studies or by video recording. Contrasted diving and foraging patterns among leatherback populations from the Pacific coasts of Costa Rica, deep-water Caribbean Sea and French Guiana shallow continental shelf suggest that leatherbacks adopt different strategies during the nesting season, most probably in response to local food occurrence and distribution. In French Guiana, trophic conditions may permit leatherbacks to compensate for reproductive costs by feeding between two consecutive nesting events. This may help to explain the relative large body size of French Guianan leatherbacks (Georges & Fossette 2006), as well as the relatively high reproductive effort and the demography of this population compared to others sites (e.g. Girondot & Fretey 1996; Girondot et al. 2007; Fossette et al., in press). However, as leatherbacks store most of their body reserves prior to reproduction, further studies and developments are required to assess individual foraging success during their pan-oceanic migrations (Myers, Lovell & Hays 2006) using more integrated techniques involving IMASEN-like technologies similar to those used successfully in the present study.

Acknowledgements We are grateful to the inhabitants of Awala-Yalimapo for their hospitality and to all those involved in the sea turtle monitoring programmes through the Direction Régionale de l’Environnement-Guyane, Kulalasi NGO, the Réserve Naturelle de l’Amana and WWF for their logistical contribution in the field. We thank A. Avril, M. Boutrif, L. Gagnon, N. Gauter, M. Hamel, N. Hanuise, J. Marin-Pache, S. Martini, M. Laur and V. Plot for their assistance in the field. Financial support for S. F. was provided in the form of a studentship from the French Ministry of Research. Funding was provided by grants to J.-Y. G. from the European Union FEDER Program, the Contrat Plan Etat Région CPER/ DocUp 2000–06, the ‘Programme Amazonie’ held by Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique. This study was carried out under CNRS-DEPE institutional licence (B67 482 18) with an individual licence attributed to to J.-Y. G. (67–220 and 04–199) and S. F. (67–256). The study adhered strictly

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 British Ecological Society, Journal of Animal Ecology, 77, 236–246

Dive and beak movement patterns in leatherbacks 245 to the legal requirements of the country in which the work was carried out and to all institutional guidelines.

References Ancel, A., Horning, M. & Kooyman, G.L. (1997) Prey ingestion revealed by oesophagus and stomach temperature recordings in cormorants. Journal of Experimental Biology, 200, 149–154. Artigas, L.F., Vendeville, P., Leopold, M., Guiral, D. & Ternon, J.-F. (2003) Marine biodiversity in French Guiana: estuarine, coastal, and shelf ecosystems under the influence of Amazonian waters. Gayana, 67, 302–326. Austin, D., Bowen, W.D., Mcmillan, J.I. & Boness, D.J. (2006) Stomach temperature telemetry reveals temporal patterns of foraging success in a free-ranging marine mammal. Journal of Animal Ecology, 75, 408–420. Baklouti, M., Devenon, J.-L., Bourret, A., Froidefond, J.-M., Ternon, J.-F. & Fuda, J.-L. (2007) New insights in the French Guiana continental shelf circulation and its relation to the North Brazil Current retroflection. Journal of Geophysical Research, 112, C02023, doi: 10.1029/2006JC003520. Boness, D.J., Bowen, W.D. & Oftedall, O.T. (1994) Evidence of a maternal foraging cycle resembling that of otariid seals in a small phocid, the harbor seal. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 34, 95–104. Booth, J. & Peters, J.A. (1972) Behavioural studies on the green turtle (Chelonia mydas) in the sea. Animal Behaviour, 20, 808–812. Boyd, I.L., Lunn, N.J. & Barton, T. (1991) Time budgets and foraging characteristics of lactating antarctic fur seals. Journal of Animal Ecology, 60, 577–592. Byers, J.A. & Moodie, J.D. (1990) Sex-specific maternal investment in pronghorn and the question of a limit on differential provisioning in ungulates. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 26, 157–164. Challet, E., Bost, C.-A., Handrich, Y., Gendner, J.-P. & Le Maho, Y. (1994) Behavioural time budget of breeding king penguins (Aptenodytes patagonica). Journal of Zoology, 233, 669–681. Delord, K., Gasco, N., Weimerskirch, H., Barbraud, C. & Micol, T. (2005) Seabird mortality in the Patagonian toothfish longline fishery around Crozet and Kerguelen Islands, 2001–03. CCAMLR Science, 12, 53–80. Durant, J.M., Massemin, S. & Handrich, Y. (2004) More eggs the better: egg formation in captive barn owls (Tyto alba). Auk, 121, 103–109. Eckert, S.A., Eckert, K.L., Ponganis, P. & Kooyman, G.L. (1989) Diving and foraging behavior of leatherback sea turtles (Dermochelys coriacea). Canadian Journal of Zoology, 67, 2834–2840. Eckert, S.A., Liew, H.-C., Eckert, K.L. & Chan, E.-H. (1996) Shallow water diving by leatherback turtles in the South China Sea. Chelonian Conservation and Biology, 2, 237–243. Fossette, S., Corbel, H., Gaspar, P., Le Maho, Y. & Georges, J.-Y. (2007a) An alternative technique for the long-term satellite tracking of Leatherback Turtles. Endangered Species Research 3. Fossette, S., Ferraroli, S., Tanaka, T., Ropert-Coudert, Y., Arai, N., Sato, K., Naito, Y., Le Maho, Y. & Georges, J.-Y. (2007b) Dispersal and dive patterns in gravid leatherback turtles during the nesting season in French Guiana. Marine Ecology Progress Series, 338, 233–247. Fossette, S., de Thoisy, B., Kelle, L., Verhage, B., Fretey, J. & Georges, J.-Y. (in press) The world’s largest leatherback rookeries: conservation and research in French Guiana, Surinam and Gabon. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology, in press. Froidefond, J.M., Gardel, L., Guiral, D., Parra, M. & Ternon, J.F. (2002) Spectral remote sensing reflectances of coastal waters in French Guiana under the Amazon influence. Remote Sensing of Environment, 80, 225–232. Gentry, R.L. & Kooyman, G.L. (1986) Fur Seals: Maternal Strategies on Land and at Sea. Princeton University Press, Princeton. Georges, J.-Y. & Fossette, S. (2006) Estimating body mass in leatherback turtles Dermochelys coriacea. Marine Ecology Progress Series, 318, 255–262. Georges, J.-Y., Fossette, S., Billes, A., Ferraroli, S., Fretey, J., Grémillet, D., Le Maho, Y., Myers, A.E., Tanaka, H. & Hays, G.C. (2007) Meta-analysis of movements in Atlantic leatherback turtles during the nesting season: conservation implications. Marine Ecology Progress Series, 338, 225–232. Georges, J.-Y. & Guinet, C. (2000) Maternal care in the subantarctic fur seals on Amsterdam island. Ecology, 81, 295–308. Girondot, M. & Fretey, J. (1996) Leatherback turtles, Dermochelys coriacea, nesting in French Guiana, 1978–1995. Chelonian Conservation Biology, 2, 204 –208. Girondot, M., Godfrey, M.H., Ponge, L. & Rivalan, P. (2007) Modeling approaches to quantify leatherback nesting trends in French Guiana and Suriname. Chelonian Conservation and Biology, 6, 37–46. Hays, G.C., Glen, F., Broderick, A.C., Godley, B.J. & Metcalfe, J.D. (2002) Behavioural plasticity in a large marine herbivore: contrasting patterns of

depth utilisation between two green turtle (Chelonia mydas) populations. Marine Biology, 141, 985–990. Hays, G.C., Isaacs, C., King, R.S., Lloyd, C. & Lovell, P. (2004) First records of oceanic dive profiles for leatherback turtles, Dermochelys coriacea, indicate behavioural plasticity associated with long-distance migration. Animal Behaviour, 67, 733–743. Hindell, M.A., Slip, D.J. & Burton, H.R. (1991) The diving behaviour of adult male and female southern elephant seals, Mirounga leonina (Pinnipedia: Phocidae). Australian Journal of Zoology, 39, 595–619. Hochscheid, S., Godley, B.J., Broderick, A.C. & Wilson, R.P. (1999) Reptilian diving: highly variable dive patterns in the green turtle Chelonia mydas. Marine Ecology Progress Series, 185, 101–112. Hochscheid, S., Maffucci, F., Bentivegna, F. & Wilson, R.P. (2004) Gulps, wheezes, and sniffs: how measurement of beak movement in sea turtles can elucidate their behaviour and ecology. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology, 316, 45–53. James, M.C. & Herman, T.B. (2001) Feeding of Dermochelys coriacea on Medusae in the Northwest Atlantic. Chelonian Conservation Biology, 4, 202–205. Jönsson, K.I. (1997) Capital and income breeding as alternative tactics of resource use in reproduction. Oikos, 78, 57–66. Kooyman, G.L., Ponganis, P.J., Castellini, M.A., Ponganis, E.P., Ponganis, K.V., Thorson, P.H., Eckert, S.A. & LeMaho, Y. (1992) Heart rates and swim speeds of emperor penguins diving under sea ice. Journal of Experimental Biology, 165, 161–180. Le Boeuf, B.J., Crocker, D.E., Costa, D.P., Blackwell, S.B., Webb, P.M. & Houser, D.S., (2000) Foraging ecology of northern elephant seals. Ecological Monographs, 70, 353–382. Le Boeuf, B.J., Naito, Y., Asaga, T., Crocker, D. & Costa, D.P. (1992) Swim speed in a female northern elephant seal: metabolic and foraging implications. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 70, 786–795. Lesage, V., Hammill, M.O. & Kovacs, K.M. (1999) Functional classification of harbor seal (Phoca vitulina) dives using depth profiles, swimming velocity, and an index of foraging success. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 77, 74 –87. Liebsch, N.S., Wilson, R.P., Bornemann, H., Adelung, D. & Plötz, J. (2007) Mouthing off about fish capture: jaw movements in pinnipeds reveal the real secrets of ingestion. Deep-Sea Research II, 54, 256–269. Luschi, P., Benhamou, S., Girard, C., Ciccione, S., Roos, D., Sudre, J. & Benvenuti, S. (2007) Marine turtles use geomagnetic cues during open-sea homing. Current Biology, 17, 126–133. Lutz, P.L. (1997) Salt, water, and pH balance in sea turtles. The Biology of Sea Turtles (eds P.L. Lutz & J.A. Musick), pp. 343–362. CRC Press, New York. Miller, J.D. (1997) Reproduction in sea turtles. The Biology of Sea Turtles (eds P.L. Lutz & J.A. Musick), pp. 51–73. CRC Press, New York. Miller, P.J.O., Johnson, M.P., Tyack, P.L. & Terray, E.A. (2004) Swimming gaits, passive drag and buoyancy of diving sperm whales Physeter macrocephalus. Journal of Experimental Biology, 207, 1953–1967. Mitani, Y., Sato, K., Ito, S., Cameron, M.F., Siniff, D.B. & Naito, Y. (2003) A method for reconstructing three-dimensional dive profiles of marine mammals using geomagnetic intensity data: results from two lactating Weddell seals. Polar Biology, 26, 311–317. Myers, A.E. & Hays, G.C. (2006) Do leatherback turtles Dermochelys coriacea forage during the breeding season? A combination of novel and traditional data-logging devices provide next insights. Marine Ecology Progress Series, 322, 259–267. Myers, A.E., Lovell, P. & Hays, G.C. (2006) Tools for studying animal behaviour: validation of dive profiles relayed via the Argos satellite system. Animal Behaviour, 71, 989–993. Plötz, J., Bornemann, H., Knust, R., Schröder, A. & Bester, M. (2001) Foraging behaviour of Weddell seals, and its ecological implications. Polar Biology, 24, 901–909. Reina, R.D., Abernathy, K.J., Marshall, G.J. & Spotila, J.R. (2005) Respiratory frequency, dive behaviour and social interactions of leatherback turtles, Dermochelys coriacea, during the inter-nesting interval. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology, 316, 1–16. Ropert-Coudert, Y., Kato, A., Liebsch, N., Wilson, R.P., Müller, G. & Baubet, E. (2004) Monitoring jaw movements: a cue to feeding activity. Game and Wildlife Science, 20, 1–19. Ropert-Coudert, Y., Liebsch, Y., Kato, A., Bedford, G., Leroy, M. & Wilson, R.P. (2002) Mouth opening in dolphins, as revealed by magnetic sensors. Isana, 36, 72–74. Saba, V.S., Santidrian-Tomillo, P., Reina, R.D., Spotila, J.R., Musick, J.A., Evans, D.A. & Paladino, F.V. (2007) The effect of the El Ninõ Southern Oscillation on the reproductive frequency of eastern Pacific leatherback. Journal of Applied Ecology, 44, 395–404.

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 British Ecological Society, Journal of Animal Ecology, 77, 236–246

246

S. Fossette et al.

Schofield, G., Katselidis, K.A., Pantis, J.D., Dimopoulos, P. & Hays, G.C. (2007) Female–female aggression: structure of interaction and outcome in loggerhead sea turtles. Marine Ecology Progress Series, 336, 267–274. Schreer, J.F., Kovacs, K.M. & O’Hara Hines, R.J. (2001) Comparative diving patterns of pinnipeds and seabirds. Ecological Monographs, 71, 137–162. Schreer, J.F. & Testa, J.W. (1996) Classification of Weddell seal diving behaviour. Marine Mammal Science, 12, 227–250. Scott, E.M., Mann, J., Watson-Capps, J.J., Sargeant, B.L. & Connor, R.C. (2005) Aggression in bottlenose dolphins: evidence for sexual coercion, male-male competition, and female tolerance through analysis of tooth-rake marks and behaviour. Behaviour, 142, 21–44. Simeone, A. & Wilson, R.P. (2003) In-depth studies of magellanic penguin (Spheniscus magellanicus) foraging: can we estimate prey consumption by perturbations in the dive profile? Marine Biology, 143, 825–831. Southwood, A.L., Andrews, D.D., Lutcavage, M.E., Paladino, F., West, N.H., George, R.H. & Jones, D.R. (1999) Heart rates and diving behavior of leatherback sea turtles in the eastern Pacific Ocean. Journal of Experimental Biology, 202, 1115 –1125. Southwood, A.L., Andrews, R.D., Paladino, F.V. & Jones, D.R. (2005) Effects of diving and swimming behaviour on body temperatures of Pacific leatherback turtles in tropical seas. Physical and Biochemical Zoology, 78, 285 –297. Stearns, S.C. (1992) The Evolution of Life Histories. Oxford University Press, Oxford. Takahashi, A., Dunn, M.J., Trathan, P.N., Croxall, J.P., Wilson, R.P., Sato, K. & Naito, Y. (2004) Krill-feeding behaviour in a chinstrap penguin Pygoscelis antarctica compared with fish-eating in magellanic penguins Speniscus magellanicus: a pilot study. Marine Ornithology, 32, 47–54.

Wallace, B.P., Cassondra, L.W., Paladino, F.V., Morreale, S.J., Lindstrom, R.T. & Spotila, J.R. (2005) Bioenergetics and diving activity of internesting leatherback turtles Dermochelys coriacea at Parque Nacional Marino Las Baulas, Costa Rica. Journal of Experimental Biology, 208, 3873–3884. Wallace, B.P., Seminoff, J.A., Kilham, S.S., Spotila, J.R. & Dutton, P.H. (2006) Leatherback turtles as oceanographic indicators: stable isotope analyses reveal a trophic dichotomy between ocean basins. Marine Biology, 149, 1432–1793. Wallace, B.P., Sotherlan, P.R., Tomillo, P.S., Reina, R.D., Spotila, J.R. & Paladino, F.V. (2007) Maternal investment in reproduction and its consequences in leatherback turtles. Oecologia, 152, 37–47. Watanabe, Y., Bornemann, H., Liebsch, N., Plötz, J., Sato, K., Naito, Y. & Miyazaki, N. (2006) Seal-mounted cameras detect invertebrate fauna on underside of Antarctic ice shelf. Marine Ecology Progress Series, 309, 297– 300. Watwood, S.L., Miller, P.J.O., Johnson, M., Madsen, P.T. & Tyack, P.L. (2006) Deep-diving foraging behaviour of sperm whales (Physeter macrocephalus). Journal of Animal Ecology, 75, 814–825. Wilson, R.P., Simeone, A., Luna-Jorquera, G., Steinfurth, A., Jackson, S. & Fahlkman, A. (2003) Patterns of respiration in diving penguins: is the last gasp an inspired tactic? Journal of Experimental Biology, 206, 1751–1763. Wilson, R.P., Steinfurth, A., Ropert-Coudret, Y., Kato, A. & Kurita, M. (2002) Lip-reading in remote subjects: an attempt to quantify and separate ingestion, breathing and vocalisation in free-living animals using penguins as a model. Marine Biology, 140, 17–27. Received 23 July 2007; accepted 16 October 2007 Handling Editor: Graeme Hayes

© 2008 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2008 British Ecological Society, Journal of Animal Ecology, 77, 236–246