Epstein (1962) The induction of nonveridical slant

Hartley, 1956; Stavrianos, 1945). In order to test this hypothesis Beck and. Gibson (195S) induced errors in the apparent slant of a target whose shape was to be ...
556KB taille 35 téléchargements 315 vues
Journal of Experimental Psychology 1962, Vol. 63, No. 5, 472-479

THE INDUCTION OF NONVERIDICAL SLANT AND THE PERCEPTION OF SHAPE J WILLIAM EPSTEIN, HELEN BONTRAGER, AND JOHN PARK University of Kansas

Koffka's (1935, pp. 224-235) explanation of shape-constancy is based on the assumption of an invariant linkage between slant and shape. Koffka's hypothesis has been formulated in the following way by Beck and Gibson (1955): "A retinal projection of a given form determines a unique relation of apparent shape to apparent slant" (p. 126). The invariance hypothesis requires that perceived shape vary as a strict function of variations in perceived slant (Langdon, 1951, 1953; Nelson & Hartley, 1956; Stavrianos, 1945). In order to test this hypothesis Beck and Gibson (195S) induced errors in the apparent slant of a target whose shape was to be judged. A triangular target cut from untextured, white cardboard was mounted on a regularly textured, vertical background, and was slanted outward from the base at an angle of 45° from the background. The target was viewed monocularly with a motionless head. Under these conditions, in the absence of binocular disparity, motion perspective, and texture gradients, the slant of the target was misperceived. The triangle assumed the slant of the background, i.e., it appeared to be perpendicular to the line of sight instead of slanted 45°. Along with this standard target, two comparison triangles placed flat on the background were exhibited. One represented a frontal parallel projection of the standard, and the other had the same physical dimensions as the standard. All 5s judged the standard triangle to be equal in 1 This investigation was supported by grants to the first author from the National Institute of Mental Health of the United States Public Health Service (M-41S3) and the General Research Fund of the University of Kansas. We wish to acknowledge the assistance of Raymond Engstrand who, together with the second author, collected the data and performed the statistical computations.

shape to the projective comparison. This match agrees perfectly with the requirements of the invariance hypothesis. When the same judgments were made with unrestricted binocular vision, 77% of the 5s selected the objectively equal comparison triangle. This result also is in the direction required by the invariance hypothesis.2 Since unrestricted observation eliminates the slant-inducing effect of the background, accurate perception of slant was restored; and, with it, veridical perception of shape was also recovered.

The general purpose of the present study was to extend and clarify Beck and Gibson's (1955) findings. The following were the major extensions and modifications: (a) The slantinduction effect was investigated when the background also was slanted from the frontal parallel plane. (b) A technique was employed which permitted continuous variation of the comparison stimulus. This made possible a more exact test of the invariance hypothesis, (c) A more accurate measure of the apparent slant of the target was obtained. These data are necessary for an analysis of the slant-shape relationship, (d) The influence of three different instructional sets on the judgment of shape was investigated. Beck and Gibson (1955) do not inform us concerning this variable, and the evidence from other studies (Gottheil & Bitterman, 1951; Klimpfinger, 1933) is fragmentary. METHOD Apparatus

The main apparatus was a rectangular light-tight tunnel 7 ft. in length with walls 2 The invariance hypothesis actually requires a 100% shift to the objectively equal comparison. 472

INDUCTION OF SLANT AND PERCEPTION OF SHAPE 20 in. wide. The interior of the tunnel was painted flat black. In the front wall of the tunnel there was an aperture which could be adjusted for monocular or binocular vision. A head-clamp chin-rest arrangement placed in front of the aperture kept S's head motionless when this was required. The two clamps restrained all but the most determined head movements, and head movements are considered highly unlikely under the conditions of the experiment. A reduction tube was inserted in the front wall which restricted the monocular field to the target and its immediate background. On the inside of the front wall was a circular fluorescent lamp (32 w., standard cool white color) which provided the only illumination in the tunnel. This lighting arrangement made invisible the shadow cast by the slanted standard. At a distance of 6 ft. from the front wall a false back wall (panel) was inserted. This panel was covered with a black and white checkerboard cloth composed of 1 X 1 in. squares. The panel could be adjusted from outside the tunnel to three degrees of slant: perpendicular to the line of sight (vertical), 20° from the perpendicular tilted away from 5 (20° A), and 20° from the perpendicular tilted toward 5 (20° T). The standard triangle was mounted in the objective center of the panel and directly in S's line of regard, and had a height of 5 in. and a base of 4 in. The adjustable comparison triangle was mounted flat on the background, 4 in. above the apex of the standard. The height and base of the comparison could be varied continuously by manipulating a set of levers located on the exterior of the roof of the tunnel. The design of this comparison stimulus is similar in essentials to the apparatus described by Gottheil and Bitterman (1951, p. 407). The comparison and standard triangles were cut from the same white cardboard of imperceptible texture. The comparison stimulus for slant was a circular disc of white cardboard mounted on a horizontal axis. The disc could be rotated on its axis, and its angle of rotation could be read directly off a protractor. The disc was located directly in S's line of sight when he turned 90° into the designated viewing position, and it was presented outside the tunnel in a normally illuminated, unrestricted field. Procedure Stimulus conditions.—The standard was judged twice by each S (one ascending and one descending trial) for each of three back-

473

ground slants. When the background was vertical or 20° T, the standard triangle was at a 45° slant from the background outward from the base. When the background was slanted 20° A, the standard was slanted 45° outward from the apex. Conditions of observation.—Each S observed the standard either monocularly or binocularly. Instructional conditions.—Three sets of instructions were used which were intended to induce different attitudes of observation. These attitudes are usually designated: the phenomenal, objective, and analytic. Each S served under one instructional condition only. The instructions were as follows: (Instructions for the phenomenal attitude) When you look into this box you will see a standard triangular target on a checkerboard background. I am interested in learning how you perceive the shape of this target. Right above the standard triangle there is a second triangle. The base and altitude of this second triangle can be varied. Your task will be to instruct me to adjust the shape of the variable triangle so that it appears to be the same shape as the standard below it. I would like you to suspend all mental judgments and give me a match which reflects your immediate perceptual impression. Don't try to figure out a good match. I want a report of your immediate perceptual impression even if you feel that the match you make would not agree with the objective physical dimensions of the target. (Instructions for the objective attitude) I am interested in learning how well you can reproduce the actual physical shape of this target. . . . This means that in the ideal case when you have completed your judgment I should be able to take the match you have made, lay it over the standard, and find that it corresponds perfectly in all dimensions. Please remember that you are to reproduce the actual physical dimensions of the target even if the match you make doesn't look equal in shape to you. To make this clear suppose you were looking at a man far in the distance. He would look very small but if you were asked to reproduce the actual size of the man you probably would be fairly accurate. This is what I wish you to do here. Reproduce the actual physical shape of the target. (Instructions for the analytic attitude) I am interested in learning how well you can reproduce the retinal shape of this target. An illustration may help make this

474

W. EPSTEIN, H. BONTRAGER, AND J. PARK

clear. If I put a coin on the table and you look directly down upon the coin, it will project a circular retinal image. However, if you stand back along the edge of the table and look at the coin from this new position, the coin will produce an elliptical retinal image. It is this retinal shape in which I am interested. . . . Your task will be to instruct me to adjust the shape of this variable triangle so that it is identical in shape with the retinal shape of the standard triangle. Remember I am interested in the retinal shape. It is not important to me whether or not the two triangles look identical in shape or are actually equal in shape when you have completed your judgment. The shape-matching procedure may now be summarized. Each 5 was instructed to make two shape-judgments of the standard for each of three background slants. The 5 made the judgments by instructing E to adjust the shape of the comparison triangle until a satisfactory match was attained. Each S performed this task under one observational condition, binocular or monocular, and under one instructional set, phenomenal, objective, or analytic. When the shape-judgments were concluded, the standard was presented again, and 5 judged the slant of the standard once for each slant of the background. All 5s were given the following instructions: Now I would like you to judge the slant of the target triangle. By "slant" I am referring to whether the triangle appears to stand straight up and down in front of you, or whether it appears to lean either toward or away from you. This is how your slant judgments will be made. First, you will look at the triangle and determine if it is slanted and, if so, how much. Outside of the box I will show you a circular disc whose slant may be varied. Your task is to adjust the slant of this disc to the same slant as the target. After you have adjusted the slant of the disc you will be given one more look into the box to check your judgment. The experiment was concluded with an interview which sought to obtain information about three aspects of the experiment: (a) the effectiveness and comprehensibility of the instructions; (6) the deliberate utilization of perceived slant during the judgment of shape; and (c) the relation between the judgments of slant obtained after the shape-

judgments were completed and the perceived slant during the shape-judgments.8

Subjects The Ss were 84 undergraduates who served in the experiment as a course requirement. They were assigned in order of their appearance to one of six groups (2 conditions of observation X 3 attitudes of observation). Thus, each of the six experimental groups contained 14 5s, about equally divided among men and women. None of the 5s knew anything about the invariance hypothesis, or the questions under investigation.

RESULTS The main results of the experiment are presented in Table 1. In all of the tables the shape data are represented as height-base (h/b) ratios. Slant-induction.'—An inspection of the mean slant judgments for monocular observation recorded in Table 1 reveals that the slant-inducing effect of the background reported by Beck and Gibson (1955) was obtained for all three background slants. As an illustration consider the fourth row of figures in Table 1. When the background was 20° A and the standard was objectively slanted 65° A, the standard was judged to be slanted 17.21° A. When the background was vertical and the standard 45° T, the standard was judged to be slanted 0.83° T. In the case of the 20° T background slant, an exaggerated slant-induction effect appears to have occurred. While the standard was objectively slanted 65° T, it was perceived to be at a slant of 6.59° T, i.e., slanted 13.41° less than the background. The comparable data for binocular vision show the expected diminution of the slant-induction effect when the 8 The use of successive rather than simultaneous slant-shape judgments can be questioned. We sought to eliminate some of the ambiguity during the interview.

INDUCTION OF SLANT AND PERCEPTION OF SHAPE

475

TABLE 1 MEAN h/b RATIOS OF COMPARISON MATCHES AND MEAN SLANT JUDGMENTS OF STANDARD Slant of the Background

20° A Observation Attitude

Comparison Mean SD h/b

Standard Mean Slant

SD

Comparison Mean SD h/b

Phenomenal Objective Analytic

.370 .397 .387

.043 10.57° A 11.40° .018 17.30° A 12,00° .097 23.57° A 13.96°

.573 .556 .530

All attitudes

.384

.127

.553

17.21° A 13.03°

Phenomenal Objective Analytic

.547 .706 .612

.177 55.72° A 9.57° .102 52.72° A 14.07° .153 56.65° A 7.91°

All attitudes

.625

.202

Objective h/b (St.) Proj. h/b (St.) Objective slant (St>

20° T

Vertical

55.03° A 11,40°

Standard Mean Slant

SD

Comparison Mean SD h/b

Ik [onociliar Visioi\ .053 0.80° T 3.37° .386 .043 0.85° T 3.58° .386 .090 1.15° A 4.75° .392

.180

0.83° T 4.43° .388

Standard Mean Slant

SD

.063 4.06° T .044 10.21° T .091 7.64° T

7.58° 6.48° 7.54°

.128

7.49°

6.59° T

Sinocu lar Vision .649 .077 20.57 °T 8.89° .584 .710 .095 17.57° T 9.45° .666 .631 .079 20.35 °T 6,74° .507

.117 25.00° T 10.63° .129 35.28° T 12.53° .134 29,35° T 11.27°

.696

.192

.213

19.49° T 7.28° .585

29.87° T 12.36°

.800 .353

.800 .585

.800 .324

65° A

45° T

65° T

• Measured in terms of the deviation from the vertical away from (A) or toward (T) S.

cues for slant were restored. The case of the 20° A background illustrates this diminution. When viewed monocularly, the target appeared to be at a slant of 17.21° A—a deviation of 47.79° from its objective slant, but only 2.79° from the slant of the background. However, when viewed binocularly, the target was judged to be at a slant of 55.03° A—a deviation of only 9.97° from its objective slant, but 35.03° from the slant of the background. In order to assess the relative magnitude of the slant-induction effect, the slant judgments were expressed as deviations from the slant of the background, and an analysis of variance was performed on these deviation scores. The analysis confirmed the observations recorded above. For any given degree of background slant the deviation was significantly greater for binocular than for monocular

vision, i.e., the slant-induction effect was greater for monocular vision. The magnitudes of the deviations were not equal for all background slants within each condition of observation. The 20° T background slant produced the greatest deviation for monocular vision and the smallest deviation for binocular vision. However, if the large deviation for monocular vision may be assumed to be an exaggerated slant-induction effect, then we can conclude that the induction effect was more pronounced when both target and background were tilted forward, Instructional sets.—An analysis of variance showed a significant interaction of attitude with condition of observation. When the target was viewed monocularly, instructional sets were ineffectual, i.e., they did not influence the shape-judgments. However, when the target was observed

476

W. EPSTEIN, H. BONTRAGER, AND J. PARK TABLE 2 THE MEAN PROJECTIVE h/b RATIO REQUIRED BY THE APPARENT SLANT OF THE STANDARD COMPARED WITH THE MEAN PROJECTIVE h/b RATIO OF THE COMPARISON SETTING Monocular

Observation Attitude

Co. Mean Proj. h/b Ratio

St. Mean Proj. h/b Ratio

Binocular Sign and % Deviation

Co. Mean Proj. h/b Ratio

St. Mean Proj. h/b Ratio

Sign and % Deviation

.674

.674 .683

-24.77 - 2.67 -20.79

.735 .732 .733

-11.56 - 3.14 -13.50

.409 .514 .460

+37.89 +24.12 + 5.00

Background Slant = 20° A Phenomenal Objective Analytic

.347 .375 .362

.200 .251 .343

+ 73.50 + 49.40 + 5.84

.507 .656 .541

Background Slant = Vertical Phenomenal Objective Analytic

.609 .601 .608

.573 .556 .529

- 5.74 - 7.65 - 12.17

.649 .710 .631

Background Slant = 20° T Phenomenal Objective Analytic

.368 .367 .375

.135 .223 .208

+172.59 + 64.12 + 79.80

binocularly, there were significant differences between the shape-judgments made under the various instructional sets. Here we will mention only that the objective attitude consistently resulted in matches which were significantly nearer to the objective dimensions of the target than the matches for the analytic and phenomenal attitudes. This finding is in agreement with the results TABLE 3 SELECTED SAMPLE OF DATA FROM 5s WHO DISPLAYED COMPLETE SLANT-INDUCTION WITH VERTICAL BACKGROUND AND MONOCULAR VISION St.

Co.

Observation N Attitude

Objective h/b Ratio

Mean Proj. h/b Ratio

10 Objective 9 Analytic Phenomenal 7 All attitudes 26

.800 .800 .800 .800

.540 .543

.555 .546

and Proj. Sign Devih/b %ation Ratio

.585 .585 .585 .585

-7.68% -7.17% -5.12% -7.85%

.562 .638 .483

reported by earlier investigators (Gottheil & Bitterman, 1951; Klimpfinger, 1933). Apparent slant-apparent shape invariance.—The following procedure was used to evaluate the invariance hypothesis. An h/b ratio was determined trigonometrically for each 5"s apparent (judged) slant of the standard at each background slant. This ratio represents the projective ratio of the shape-match demanded for that degree of apparent slant by strict adherence to the invariance requirements. Next, the projective h/b ratio was determined for S's setting of the comparison triangle at each background slant. A comparison of these two ratios allowed us to evaluate the degree of slant-shape invariance. Any difference between the two ratios represents a deviation from invariance requirements. The

INDUCTION OF SLANT AND PERCEPTION OF SHAPE

477

results of our analysis are summarized slanted 20° T, although errors of considerable magnitude were obtained in Table 2. To clarify the logic of this analysis with the background 20° A. With a selected sample of data has been only one exception, the deviations presented in Table 3. The mean h/b obtained with monocular observation ratios contained in Table 3 were ob- under the 20° A and 20° T background tained from 5s who manifested total conditions were at least twice as great slant induction when viewing the as the deviations obtained for binocular standard monocularly against a ver- vision. A review of the ratio pairings for the tical background. In this special case, when the standard is perceived individual 5s (252 pairings, 126 for to be perpendicular to the line of sight, monocular and the same number for the frontal parallel projection for the binocular vision) confirms these obapparent slant of the target is in servations. There was not one case effect the actual projective shape of perfect correspondence between of the target. This is to say that, if the two h/b ratios. Table 4, which the invariance requirements are to be presents the distribution of deviations strictly satisfied, the projective h/b according to sign, reflects this fact. Verbal reports.—The information ratio of the comparison (which in this special case is identical with the derived from the interview tended to objective h/b ratio of the comparison) confirm the validity of the experiAll Ss gave must equal the projective h/b ratio mental operations. of the standard. For the dimen- evidence of having understood the sions of our standard this means task which was posed by the instructhat a perfect invariance fit would be TABLE 4 h/b = 0.585. However, consulting the fourth column in Table 3, we find DISTRIBUTION OF DEVIATIONS FROM INVARIANCE REQUIREMENTS FOR MONOCULAR that this theoretical value was not AND BINOCULAR VISION attained. The deviations between the means were not great, but they Slant of Background were present, nonetheless. All Slants An inspection of Table 2 reveals Devia20° A Vertical 20° T Observation tions Devia- Devia- DeviaAttitude that in no instance did the mean protions tions tions jective h/b ratio of the comparison setting satisfy the invariance require+ - + + - + ments. The deviations obtained for Monocular Vision monocular observation ranged from -5.74% to 172.59%. For binocular Phenomenal 12 2 3 11 13 1 28 14 vision the range of deviations was Objective 10 4 2 12 12 2 24 18 8 6 3 11 13 1 24 18 - 2.67% to 37.89%. Relatively small Analytic deviations from invariance were ob- All attitudes 30 12 8 34 38 4 76 50 tained when the background was Binocular Vision vertical. In addition, there was no significant difference between the Phenomenal 3 11 3 11 1.1 3 17 25 grand mean deviations for monocular Objective 6 8 8 6 11 3 25 17 3 11 2 12 8 6 13 29 and binocular vision with the vertical Analytic background. The greatest deviations All attitudes 12 30 13 29 30 12 55 71 occurred when the background was -

478

W. EPSTEIN, H. BONTRAGER, AND J. PARK

tions. Very few 5s said that they had made deliberate efforts to take slant into account while making shapejudgments. All 5s agreed that the judgments of slant made at the end of the experiment corresponded well with the apparent slant of the target during the earlier judgments of shape. Only a few 5s expressed any strong lack of confidence in their judgments. DISCUSSION The results of this experiment confirmed and extended Beck and Gibson's (1955) findings concerning the slantinduction effect. The effect was observed to obtain not only when the background was vertical, but also when the background was slanted 20° A or 20° T. In fact, there is some reason for concluding that the effect was more pronounced when the background was slanted forward. We have no explanation for this latter finding, nor do we see any way of deriving it from the general stimulus-gradient theory. The results with regard to the invariance hypothesis agreed with Beck and Gibson's findings in their general aspects, but were sufficiently different to warrant comment. The main point of disagreement was this: For both monocular and binocular observation our results showed much less adherence to the invariance requirements than did the results of Beck and Gibson. For instance, Beck and Gibson report that when the standard was viewed monocularly with a stationary head, all of their 30 5s selected a protective match, while with unrestricted binocular vision 23 of 30 5s selected an objective match. None of our 5s made a perfect projective or objective match when these were demanded by the apparent slant. The main reason for this disagreement resides in the difference between the measurement techniques employed in the two experiments.4 We used a con4

Beck and Gibson (1955, p. 131) recognized the limitations of their technique for a precise test of the invariance hypothesis.

tinuously variable comparison which enabled 5 to make sensitive discriminations. The method which Beck and Gibson used forced 5 to choose between one of two extreme alternatives, i.e., objective or projective. There was no way 5 could indicate any other perceived shape. In this situation 5 would probably select the comparison triangle which was most like the one he perceived even when he recognized that the two stimuli were not identical. Thus, all perceived shapes which clustered about the projective comparison were designated by that comparison and, similarly, all apparent shapes which clustered about the objective comparison were designated by that comparison. Lacking an opportunity to differentiate in the response system, 5s produced results which appeared to reflect agreement in the perceptual system. The evidence from past studies with regard to the effect of attitudes on the perception of shape is difficult to interpret. Klimpfinger (1933), who is most frequently cited, did not actually compare the influence of different attitudes within the same experimental situation. Instead, his conclusions were based mainly on a comparison of his data with the results of other Es. Gottheil and Bitterman (1951) used the same 5s for each of the three attitudes. There is no way of knowing what effect judgments made under a previous attitude may have had on the later judgments obtained for another attitude. In addition, Gottheil and Bitterman do not tell us whether 5 viewed the target monocularly or binocularly. Our own results suggest an interaction of attitude with condition of observation. In light of the paucity of evidence on this question further research is needed.

SUMMARY The experiment had three aspects: (a) to determine whether the slant-induction effect reported by Beck and Gibson (1955) would obtain when the background was slanted; (6) to test more precisely the slantshape invariance hypothesis; and (c) to investigate the influence of three attitudes of

INDUCTION OF SLANT AND PERCEPTION OF SHAPE observation on the perception of shape under monocular and binocular vision. The main results were: (a) The slantinducing effect of the background was observed when the background was slanted 20° from the perpendicular either away from 5 (20° A) or toward 5 (20° T) in addition to the case in which the background was perpendicular to the line of sight. (6) Under no condition did the judgments of apparent slant and apparent shape covary exactly as demanded by the invariance hypothesis. The deviations from invariance ranged from —24.77% to 172.59%. (c) The influence of attitudes on the perception of shape was restricted to binocular viewing. Attitudes were found to be ineffectual in determining perceived shape when the standard was viewed monocularly.

REFERENCES BECK, J., & GIBSON, J. J. The relation of apparent shape to apparent slant in the perception of objects. /. exp. Psychol., 1955, SO, 125-133.

GOTTHEIL, E., & BlTTERMAN, M. E.

479 The

measurement of shape-constancy. Amer. J. Psychol, 1951, 64, 406^08. KLIMPFINGER, S. Uber den Einfluss von intentionaler Einstellung und Ubung auf die Gestaltkonstanz. Arch. ges. Psychol., 1933, 88, 551-598. KOFFKA, K. Principles of Gestalt psychology. New York: Harcourt, Brace, 1935. LANGDON, F. J. The perception of changing shape. Quart. J. exp. Psychol., 1951, 3, 157-165. LANGDON, F. J. Further studies in the perception of a changing shape. Quart. J. exp' Psychol,, 1953, S, 89-107. NELSON, T. M., & HARTLEY, S. H. The perception of form in an unstructured field. J. gen. Psychol, 1956, 54, 57-63. STAVRIANOS, B. K. The relation of shapeperception to explicit judgments of inclination. Arch. Psychol, N. Y., 1945, No. 296. (Received March 20, 1961)