Edward G. Freedman

Oct 22, 2008 - The transformation of sensory information into motor ac- tions is a ... The costs of publication of this article were defrayed in part by the payment.
1MB taille 4 téléchargements 326 vues
Edward G. Freedman J Neurophysiol 100:3375-3393, 2008. First published Oct 22, 2008; doi:10.1152/jn.90669.2008 You might find this additional information useful... This article cites 49 articles, 23 of which you can access free at: http://jn.physiology.org/cgi/content/full/100/6/3375#BIBL Updated information and services including high-resolution figures, can be found at: http://jn.physiology.org/cgi/content/full/100/6/3375 Additional material and information about Journal of Neurophysiology can be found at: http://www.the-aps.org/publications/jn

This information is current as of August 31, 2010 .

Downloaded from jn.physiology.org on August 31, 2010

Journal of Neurophysiology publishes original articles on the function of the nervous system. It is published 12 times a year (monthly) by the American Physiological Society, 9650 Rockville Pike, Bethesda MD 20814-3991. Copyright © 2008 by the American Physiological Society. ISSN: 0022-3077, ESSN: 1522-1598. Visit our website at http://www.the-aps.org/.

J Neurophysiol 100: 3375–3393, 2008. First published October 22, 2008; doi:10.1152/jn.90669.2008.

Coupling Between Horizontal and Vertical Components of Saccadic Eye Movements During Constant Amplitude and Direction Gaze Shifts in the Rhesus Monkey Edward G. Freedman Department of Neurobiology and Anatomy; University of Rochester Medical Center; University of Rochester; Rochester, New York Submitted 12 June 2008; accepted in final form 15 October 2008

The transformation of sensory information into motor actions is a critical function of the nervous system, and one that is not yet fully understood. The oculomotor system continues to be a model for the study of the neural control of behavior in large part because of the stereotyped nature of saccadic eye movements and because the neural elements controlling these high-velocity, conjugate movements of the eyes are accessible for experimentation. Careful studies of saccadic behavior have constrained hypotheses concerning their neural control and led to direct tests of the predictions of various oculomotor control models. Analyses of gaze shifts made when the head is free to move will similarly constrain hypotheses concerned with the coordination of the eyes and head during visual orienting behaviors.

Saccadic eye movements change the direction of the line of sight and permit new images to fall on the receptor rich foveal region of the retina. They are defined by a set of characteristic relationships between amplitude, duration, and peak velocity. These stereotyped relationships (sometimes referred to as the “main sequence”) have been described in detail during horizontal, vertical, and oblique movements in a variety of species (Bahill et al. 1975; Baloh et al. 1975; Becker and Ju¨rgens 1990; Collewijn et al. 1988a,b; Evinger et al. 1981; Fuchs 1967; Guitton and Mandl 1980; King et al. 1986; Smit et al. 1990; van Gisbergen et al. 1985; Yee et al. 1985). For example, the positive correlation between saccade duration and amplitude during horizontal saccades is illustrated in Fig. 1A. In this panel, the durations of horizontal saccades made to targets displaced by 5, 15, 35, and 50° are plotted. In the diagram below (Fig. 1C), these movements are represented schematically by arrows of different lengths along the abscissa. As shown in A, horizontal saccades (made by this non-human primate subject) having amplitudes of 35° take around 75 ms to complete. The durations of vertical saccades with amplitudes of 20° (shown schematically in Fig. 1C by the black arrow along the ordinate), were ⬃50 ms (Fig. 1B, unfilled circles). If horizontal and vertical saccade components were independently generated, when oblique movements had 20° vertical components and 35° horizontal components, movements could be dramatically curved (Fig. 1C, gray trajectory) because the 20° vertical component would be completed 25 ms before the completion of the 35° horizontal component. However, during oblique saccades, the horizontal and vertical components tend to begin and end at nearly the same times and trajectories of oblique saccades tend to be straight (Fig. 1C, dotted lines). As shown in Fig. 1B, this is a result of the temporal stretching of the smaller-amplitude component (the vertical component in this example). Instead of taking 50 ms for the eyes to move vertically 20°, when part of an oblique saccade with 35 or 50° horizontal components, the 20° vertical component took 75 or 100 ms to complete: about the same amount of time it took to complete the 35 or 50° horizontal component. The duration of the vertical component is increased to match closely the duration of the horizontal component even though the amplitude of the vertical component is not changing. This results in a positive correlation between vertical saccade duration and the amplitude of the horizontal saccade component (Fig. 1B).

Address for reprint requests and other correspondence: Dept. of Neurobiology and Anatomy; University of Rochester Medical Center; University of Rochester; Box 603, Rochester, NY 14642.

The costs of publication of this article were defrayed in part by the payment of page charges. The article must therefore be hereby marked “advertisement” in accordance with 18 U.S.C. Section 1734 solely to indicate this fact.

INTRODUCTION

www.jn.org

0022-3077/08 $8.00 Copyright © 2008 The American Physiological Society

3375

Downloaded from jn.physiology.org on August 31, 2010

Freedman EG. Coupling between horizontal and vertical components of saccadic eye movements during constant amplitude and direction gaze shifts in the rhesus monkey. J Neurophysiol 100: 3375–3393, 2008. First published October 22, 2008; doi:10.1152/jn.90669.2008. When the head is free to move, changes in the direction of the line of sight (gaze shifts) can be accomplished using coordinated movements of the eyes and head. During repeated gaze shifts between the same two targets, the amplitudes of the saccadic eye movements and movements of the head vary inversely as a function of the starting positions of the eyes in the orbits. In addition, as head-movement amplitudes and velocities increase, saccade velocities decline. Taken together these observations lead to a reversal in the expected correlation between saccade duration and amplitude: small-amplitude saccades associated with large head movements can have longer durations than larger-amplitude saccades associated with small head movements. The data in this report indicate that this reversal occurs during gaze shifts along the horizontal meridian and also when considering the horizontal component of oblique saccades made when the eyes begin deviated only along the horizontal meridian. Under these conditions, it is possible to determine whether the variability in the duration of the constant amplitude vertical component of oblique saccades is accounted for better by increases in horizontal saccade amplitude or increases in horizontal saccade duration. Results show that vertical saccade duration can be inversely related to horizontal saccade amplitude (or unrelated to it) but that horizontal saccade duration is an excellent predictor of vertical saccade duration. Modifications to existing hypotheses of gaze control are assessed based on these new observations and a mechanism is proposed that can account for these data.

3376

E. G. FREEDMAN

A

B Saccades Along the Horizontal Meridian

110 Vertical Saccade Duration (ms)

Horizontal Saccade Duration (ms)

110 90 70 50 30

-10 0 30 50 Horizontal Saccade Amplitude (deg)

90 70 50 30 -10 0 30 50 Horizontal Saccade Amplitude (deg)

Velocity

20 10 0 0 20

40

Time

Horizontal

Vertical Velocity (deg/s)

E 800 r2 = 0.99

600

FIG. 1. A: saccade duration plotted as a function of amplitude for movements directed along the horizontal meridian when the head is restrained. B: vertical saccade duration plotted as a function of horizontal component amplitude during oblique, head restrained saccadic eye movements. Vertical saccade amplitude did not vary and was 20°. For comparison (unfilled circles) duration of vertical saccades of 20° during pure vertical movements are also shown. C: arrows along the abscissa represent movements of various amplitudes directed along the horizontal meridian. Arrow along the ordinate represents vertical movements of constant amplitude. Gray line indicates the curved trajectory of a hypothetical movement that might occur if vertical component duration were not increased to match that of the horizontal component. Dashed lines represent more realistic saccade trajectories during oblique movements that combine varying horizontal amplitudes with constant vertical amplitude. D: schematic diagram of velocity profiles of vertical (black) and horizontal (dashed, gray) components that lead to curved saccade trajectories. E: vertical velocity is plotted as a function of horizontal velocity for two oblique saccades. This panel presents a method for assessing saccade curvature. Low curvature (black) results in high VAF; high curvature (gary) results in low VAF.

400 r2 = 0.50

200 0 0

200 400 600 800 Horizontal Velocity (deg/s)

In models of the head-restrained saccadic system, component durations are not explicitly determined or controlled. Instead movement durations are determined by the desired size of the movement and by the exponential function that converts motor error into a signal proportional to velocity (known as the saccadic burst generator). In these models, the tight link between saccade duration and amplitude stems from the presumed stability of the burst generator function for movements in a given direction. As the relative amplitudes of the horizontal and vertical components change (i.e., movement directions differ), there are two ways to alter the durations of saccade components. The input to the burst generator or the burst generator function itself can be altered as a function of saccade direction. Both methods can change the duration of a constant J Neurophysiol • VOL

amplitude saccade component such that the smaller-amplitude component is stretched in time to match the duration of the larger component (see Fig. 1B). As indicated, matching the durations of the horizontal and vertical components is necessary if saccade curvature is to be avoided. However, curved saccades like that shown schematically in Fig. 1C (gray line) will also result if the horizontal component is delayed with respect to the vertical. An example of this is illustrated in Fig. 1D, which shows a schematic diagram of vertical velocity (black) plotted as a function of time. Consider the movements that would result if this velocity profile was paired with a horizontal movement that began after a delay (gray dashed velocity profile) or if it was paired with a horizontal movement that accelerated and decelerated along a different trajectory

100 • DECEMBER 2008 •

www.jn.org

Downloaded from jn.physiology.org on August 31, 2010

D

C Vertical

Vertical and Oblique Saccades with 20 Vertical Components

COUPLING DURING OBLIQUE GAZE SHIFTS

J Neurophysiol • VOL

H me/共1 ⫹ c*Vme兲

(1)

Where Hme is horizontal motor error, Vme is vertical motor error, and c is a coupling constant (on the order of 0.05). This approach has proven to be reasonably successful because of the seemingly immutable relationship between saccade duration and amplitude; the degree to which the horizontal velocity must be reduced so that its duration matches that of the vertical component can be calculated given the amplitude of the vertical component and knowledge of the duration-amplitude relationship. The same can be said about common-source models with the caveat that the duration-amplitude relationship for movements made in a particular direction must be known to accurately predict the angular constant that scales component velocities. When the head is free to move, changes in the direction of the line of sight can be accomplished using coordinated movements of both the eyes and head. Two observations concerning head unrestrained gaze shifts are pertinent. The first is that during gaze shifts having similar amplitudes, the relative contributions of the eyes and head vary inversely and depend on the starting positions of the eyes in the orbits (Delreux et al. 1991; Freedman and Sparks 1997a,b, 2000; Stahl 1999, 2001; Tomlinson 1990; Volle and Guitton 1993). The second is that as head-movement amplitudes and velocities increase, the velocities of the associated saccadic eye movements decline (Freedman 2001, 2005; Freedman and Sparks 2000; Tomlinson 1990; Tomlinson and Bahra 1986). A direct consequence of these two observations is that during gaze shifts of the same amplitude, initiated with the eyes in different orbital positions, the relationship between saccade duration and amplitude can be reversed: the linear relationship has a negative slope (see RESULTS). Given this uncoupling of the usually tight link between saccade amplitude and duration, it is possible to test the hypothesis that it is the relative amplitudes of horizontal and vertical components of oblique saccades that leads to the observed component stretching. Under the conditions outlined in the preceding text, the effects of saccade component duration and amplitude on the orthogonal component duration can be separately assessed. In the following text, it is demonstrated that the duration of the constant amplitude vertical component of oblique saccades does not depend on the amplitude of the horizontal component. These data will help constrain hypotheses that propose mechanisms for the control of coordinated eye-head movements. METHODS

Two female rhesus monkeys weighing between 4.5 and 6 kg (Macaca mulatta) served as subjects. Each had a scleral eye coil implanted in one eye (Judge et al. 1980) and a small head-restraint post secured to the skull. Surgical procedures were carried out using general inhalation anesthesia (isoflourane) and aseptic techniques. After full recovery, subjects were trained to make gaze shifts to visual targets for a juice reward. All surgical and experimental procedures were approved by the University of Rochester Committee on Animal Resources and comply with the National Institutes of Health guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals. Subjects sat in a primate chair that prevented movements of the hips and shoulders but permitted unrestricted horizontal and vertical movements of the head. The chair was positioned such that the head of the monkey was located at the center of a 1.2-m cube that housed 3 pairs

100 • DECEMBER 2008 •

www.jn.org

Downloaded from jn.physiology.org on August 31, 2010

(gray solid line). In both of these hypothetical cases, the majority of the vertical component of the saccade would be accomplished before an equivalent amount of the horizontal component was completed, leading to dramatically curved saccades (Fig. 1C, gray line). The lack of dramatically curved oblique saccades when the head is restrained suggests that the horizontal and vertical saccade components begin and end together and component velocity profiles differ only in scale. In Fig. 1E, a method for comparing curvature of different saccades is presented. Vertical velocity is plotted as a function of horizontal velocity for a head restrained saccade (black). Ninety-nine percent of the variance in vertical velocity is accounted for by the horizontal velocity; an indication that vertical and horizontal velocity differ only in scale. The variance accounted for (VAF) will be much lower if movements are curved (Fig. 1E, gray trace). This method will be revisited when the straightness of oblique gaze shifts is addressed. Several mechanisms have been proposed to account for the coupling of horizontal and vertical components of oblique saccades (Becker and Ju¨rgens 1990; Grossman and Robinson 1988; Nichols and Sparks 1996; Quaia and Optican 1997; Smit et al. 1990; van Gisbergen et al. 1985; van Opstal and Kappen 1993). Although there are differences among them, each of these hypotheses (except the proposal of Grossman and Robinson 1988) uses the direction of the saccade (or equivalently the relative amplitudes of the horizontal and vertical components) to scale the component velocities. Two classes of model have been proposed. Common source models (Quaia and Optican 1997; van Gisbergen et al. 1985; van Opstal and Kappen 1993) rely on a single saccadic burst generator that produces a signal proportional to vectorial velocity. Because both components arise from a common velocity command, a critical prediction of this hypothesis is that horizontal and vertical velocities will differ only by a scaling factor, thus guaranteeing that movements will be straight (in a variant of this type of model, Quaia and Optican 1997 suggest that common source models may be consistent with directionspecific curvature). To accomplish appropriate velocity scaling, common-source models calculate movement direction and adjust both the gain (asymptotic value) and the time constant of the exponential function that defines the relationship between saccade component size and peak velocity (see van Gisbergen et al. 1985, Eq. 3). A critical prediction of common-source models is that the slopes of the duration-amplitude relationships will be determined by the direction of movement. Note that all the above-referenced models were designed to account for head-restrained saccade data. Eye-head interactions are irrelevant under these conditions. The predictions of these models might differ if they were extended to include movements of the head. Cross-coupling models assume that a vectorial command is first decomposed into separate horizontal and vertical components and that these serve as inputs to separate, interacting burst generators (Becker and Ju¨rgens 1990; Grossman and Robinson 1988). To account for component stretching of oblique saccades, the cross-coupling model of Becker and Ju¨rgens (1990) scales the inputs to the burst generators according to movement direction. This hypothesis proposes that the input to the horizontal burst generator is given by the following equation

3377

E. G. FREEDMAN

of magnetic field coils (CNC Engineering, Seattle, WA). The four vertical faces of the cube (front-back and left-right) contained two pairs of Helmholtz coils in spatial and phase quadrature (Collewijn 1977). The two horizontal faces of the cube (top and bottom) contained a third pair of Helmholtz coils for measurement of the vertical angle of the scleral and head-mounted coils. A five-pole low-pass Bessel filter with a cutoff frequency of 3 kHz was used to remove the coil system carrier frequency. A second low-pass filter (time constant ⫽ 0.3 ms) conditioned coil signals before digitizing. This filter will introduce no more than a 0.2-ms delay in signals with frequency content between 0 and 530 Hz. A lightweight cam-lock device was secured to the implanted head post. This device carried a coil similar to that implanted in the eye to measure head position. In addition, three small laser diodes (red: 650 nm) were secured to the head daily. The central laser was aligned with the midsagittal plane of the subject’s head, the second laser was directed ⬃18° to the left of center, and the third was directed ⬃18° to the right. Illumination of the head-mounted laser was controlled via custom behavioral control software.

Visual targets were presented by pointing green laser diodes (532 nm) at the inside of a 1.5-m-diam hemisphere painted 18% gray. The hemisphere (0.5-in acrylic; Capital Plastics, Beltsville, MD) was positioned so that the center of its vertical face was aligned with the geometric center of the field coil frame. Positioning of the lasers was carried out by two independent, two-axis, motorized gimbals (custom designed using pairs of RGV 100 rotation stages; Newport, Irvine, CA). Each pair of rotation stages, arranged in a Fick gimbal, could direct a laser spot at any location within the hemisphere with better than 0.01° accuracy and precision rated at 0.0003°. Switching lasers on and off was controlled via custom data acquisition and behavioral control software running on a PC with an extended PCI bus (National Instruments; Austin TX).

Behavioral tasks Subjects were trained to perform the following task. Before the onset of each trial, both two-axis gimbals were positioned such that the lasers were pointed at locations randomly selected from a set of potential target locations. At this stage, neither target was illuminated. Trials began with the illumination of the first target [in these data, this was always located at (0,0)—a target aligned with the mid-sagittal plane of the subject’s body and equidistant from the upper and lower bounds of the display]. In conjunction with the illumination of the first target, one of the three head-mounted lasers was randomly selected by computer and also turned on. Subjects were trained to look at the green fixation target while simultaneously aligning the red headmounted laser with this target spot (within a computer defined window: radius 4°). When the central head-mounted laser was selected, the eyes were positioned in the center of the orbits (the eyes and head were aligned). When the left-pointing head laser was illuminated, alignment of the head laser and the target spot required the subject to fixate the central target while rotating the head to the right – positioning the eyes to the left in the orbits. Similarly when the rightpointing head laser was lit, the eyes began deviated to the right in the orbits. After fixating the first target and aligning the head appropriately for a variable interval (250 –1,250 ms; 50-ms increments), a second green target was turned on. This spot could be in any of 24 locations and was randomly selected before the beginning of each trial. After a variable delay interval (250 –1,000 ms; 50-ms increments) when head alignment and fixation of the first target had to be maintained, the first target and the head mounted laser were turned off (the secondary green laser spot was left on). This served as the cue for the subject to initiate a gaze shift to the location of the still illuminated target. J Neurophysiol • VOL

Data Horizontal and vertical gaze and head position data were sampled at 1 kHz and eye position relative to the head was calculated by subtracting head from gaze at each time sample. Eye position is defined as the position of the eyes relative to the head and rapid eye movements relative to the head are referred to as saccadic eye movements or saccades throughout the text. Gaze and head positions are defined relative to an external/spatial reference. Data were stored and analyzed off-line using Matlab (Natick, MA). Velocities were

30 20 Initial Head Alignment Locations

10 0

Initial Fixation

-10 -20 -30 -30

-20 -10 0 10 20 Horizontal Position of Visual Targets (deg)

30

FIG. 2. The locations of the majority of visual targets used during head unrestrained data collection (other targets specified in the text). The initial fixation target was always located centrally (0,0), and 1 of 3 head-mounted lasers was required to be aligned with this central target. As a result the head could be pointed initially in one of 3 directions (starred locations). Initial head locations are displaced vertically for display only.

100 • DECEMBER 2008 •

www.jn.org

Downloaded from jn.physiology.org on August 31, 2010

Visual target presentation

Subjects had 500 ms to initiate and complete gaze shifts to the second target and were required to fixate this location within a computer defined window (2.5–5° radius). Task contingencies were implemented in real time with 1-ms resolution. Only trials during which subjects met the spatial and temporal requirements of the task are included in this report. Figure 2 illustrates in schematic form the positions of the 24 targets (filled circles) and the initial fixation target location (central square). Several additional target locations were added along the horizontal meridian and will be discussed explicitly in the text. In addition, the three possible positions of the head are also shown. It is important to remember that the task required the head-mounted laser to be aligned with the central fixation target; only the head-mounted laser and the central target were illuminated at the beginning of each trial. However, because the head-mounted lasers were pointed in three different directions; to align these targets, the head had to be rotated in the horizontal plane. As a result the head began directed toward one of the three locations indicated in Fig. 2 (for display purposes head alignment positions are slightly elevated in this figure; actual rotations of the head were along the horizontal meridian). The eyes were not deviated vertically in the orbits and always began in a central position. The vertical component of gaze shifts reported below was restricted in amplitude to be within 35° of the horizontal meridian. Because the eyes began in a vertically centered orbital position, vertical head contributions did not exceed 5° (head movements were primarily horizontal). A consequence of this arrangement was that for each presented target, the vertical components of oblique gaze shifts and saccadic eye movements were relatively constant, whereas the horizontal eye- and head-movement components varied.

Vertical Position of Visual Targets (deg)

3378

COUPLING DURING OBLIQUE GAZE SHIFTS

RESULTS

Data for this report include 12,672 head-unrestrained gaze shifts from subject P and 10,334 from subject S. Additional head-restrained data from each subject were collected separately and are included for comparison as indicated in the text. Because leftward and rightward as well as upward and downward movements were made, unless explicitly stated in the text, data are presented as the absolute value of movement amplitude. Negative amplitudes and velocities indicate leftward (or downward) movements. When the head is free to move, gaze shifts of a particular amplitude and direction can be composed of movements of the eyes and head. During repeated gaze shifts to the same target, eye- and head-movement amplitudes vary inversely as a function of the starting positions of the eyes in the orbits (Delreux et al. 1991; Freedman and Sparks 1997b; Stahl 1999, 2001; Volle and Guitton 1993). For example, a 35° gaze shift might be accomplished by making a 35° saccade. The head movement, if any, would occur after the gaze shift was completed, so on such a trial, the head contribution would be 0°. However, the same 35° gaze shift might be made up of a 10° saccade coupled with a large (and fast) head movement that contributes 25° to the overall change in gaze position. The former movement is likely to be observed when the eyes begin deviated in the orbits away from the direction of the gaze shift, whereas the latter example might occur when the eyes were initially deviated in the direction of the ensuing gaze shift. Gaze shifts along the horizontal meridian Before presenting data from oblique gaze shifts, it is important to consider the effects of head movements on the amplitude and duration of saccadic eye movements when repeated J Neurophysiol • VOL

gaze shifts are directed to the same visual target displaced along the horizontal meridian. Because gaze amplitudes are similar when movements are made between the same two visual targets, as head contribution to gaze shifts increases, the amplitude of saccadic eye movements necessarily declines. Figure 3 plots data from gaze shifts made to a target displaced 35° to the left of fixation. Using the head-pointing task (METHODS), initial eye positions varied from 19.6° in the direction of the gaze shift to 23.2° away from movement direction. During 526 gaze shifts (Fig. 3A; subject P) the amplitude of saccades varied over a fairly large range (5–35°), even while gaze shift amplitude varied very little (absolute value of mean horizontal gaze amplitude ⫽ 35.2 ⫾ 1.6°; mean vertical gaze amplitude ⫽ 1.5 ⫾ 1.1°). For this set of movements, small saccadic eye movements were part of gaze shifts with large, fast head movements, whereas larger saccades were coupled with smaller, slower head movements. As demonstrated previously for both horizontal (Freedman and Sparks 2000) and vertical (Freedman 2005) gaze shifts, as head velocity and head contribution to the gaze shift increase, saccade velocity declines. A direct consequence of these observations is that during constant amplitude gaze shifts in which the amplitude of the saccade varies, the duration-amplitude relationship for saccadic eye movements is reversed. In Fig. 3, horizontal saccade duration is plotted as a function of the amplitude of the saccade (note that movements were to the left, but data are plotted as the absolute value of saccade amplitude). As shown the relationship between saccade duration and amplitude has a negative slope. To provide some insight into the reversal of the durationamplitude relationship during horizontal gaze shifts having similar amplitudes, horizontal gaze (black), eye (dark gray), and head (light gray) velocities are plotted as functions of time for three sets of movements (n ⫽ 5). Each set of trials was selected from a different section of the data shown in Fig. 3A (arrows indicate regions). In Fig. 3B, head-movement amplitudes were large (total head-movement amplitudes were between 45 and 49°; head contributions ranged from 28 to 30°), trials in Fig. 3C were associated with head movements between 24 and 26°, and movements in D had head movement amplitudes between 5 and 10°. Figure 3E plots data similar to that shown in A but for the second subject (S). In each case, smaller saccades associated with large head contributions were slower than larger saccades associated with small head contributions, reversing the relationship between saccade duration and amplitude observed when the head is restrained (e.g., Fig. 1B). The slopes of the lines of best fit for these data were ⫺2.5 (Fig. 3A) and ⫺2.7 (B). During horizontal gaze shifts to targets located to the left and right, displaced 25, 30, 35, 40, or 50° from the fixation target (Fig. 4), the slopes of the duration-amplitude relationships were negative for both subjects S (gray) and P (black). During movements to targets displaced ⬎20°, slopes ranged from ⫺2.4 to ⫺4.7. Figure 4B provides in tabular form the r2, slope and number of trials for both subjects. Also shown in Fig. 4 are the lines of best fit for the duration-amplitude relationships observed during gaze shifts to a 20° target. In this case, the slopes were not significantly different from zero and positive. Note that during 20° gaze shifts with the eyes deviated in the direction of the movement, head contribution remained quite small (0.19 ⫾ 0.3°). As a result, horizontal saccade amplitudes did not vary over a large range (18.9 ⫾ 2.3°) and the duration

100 • DECEMBER 2008 •

www.jn.org

Downloaded from jn.physiology.org on August 31, 2010

calculated using a five-point parabolic function (see Freedman (2005) for description). The beginning and end of horizontal and vertical gaze, head and eye movements were calculated using velocity and acceleration criteria. To be marked as the onset of a movement, samples had to have velocities that exceeded 30°/s or accelerations that exceeded 5,000°/s2 for each of three consecutive samples. Movement end was marked when velocity and acceleration fell below these levels. Each movement was individually inspected and criteria marked correctly nearly 98% of all gaze, eye, and head movements. Our criteria failed to mark movements when both horizontal and vertical gaze (or eye) velocities fell to 0 and then resumed to complete the trial within the time requirements of the task. These trials consisted of essentially two movements separated by a very short interval and were not analyzed. Beginning and end of horizontal and vertical gaze, eye, and head movements were calculated separately; however, both the beginning and end of eye movements and gaze shifts differed very little using the criteria in the preceding text. To be specific, gaze shift end rarely (⬍1% of trials) exceeded eye movement end by more than ⬃10 ms; eye movements that reached position plateaus and maintained constant position for extended periods while gaze shifts continued were not observed. Durations were calculated as the time from beginning to end, amplitudes were calculated as the change in position during the movement. Head contribution is defined as the change in position of the head that occurs during the gaze shift. Statistical null hypotheses [Kolmogorov-Smirnoff (K-S) or Student’s t-test] were rejected when P ⬍ 0.05. Slopes of durationamplitude relationships were determined to be statistically different from 0 by calculating 95% confidence intervals and determining whether these excluded the 0-slope line.

3379

3380

E. G. FREEDMAN

A

Horizontal Gaze Shifts

B 0 -200 -600

Horizontal Velocity (deg/s)

Horizontal Eye Movement Duration (ms)

200

150

100

Slope = -2.5 n = 526 r2 = 0.47 Subject P

0

C

-200

-800

D -200

Gaze Eye Head

-800 100 200 300 400 500 600

Time (ms)

E

150

100 Slope = -2.7 n = 196 r2 = 0.42 Subject S

50

0

5 10 15 20 25 30 35 Horizontal Eye Movement Amplitude (deg)

40

of saccades changed very little. This results in lack of correlation between duration and amplitude during movements to 20° targets. If the range of initial eye positions had been increased so that saccade amplitudes varied over a wider range, it is likely that the slope of the duration-amplitude relationship would have been negative during movements to these targets as it was during gaze shifts to the targets displaced by ⱖ25°. Oblique gaze shifts Before describing the overall effects of head movements on horizontal and vertical saccade components, several single trial examples are illustrated in Fig. 5 to provide an overall impression of the results. In Fig. 5A, vertical gaze (A), eye (B), and head (C) positions are plotted as functions of horizontal positions during each of these three movements; note that head positions are plotted for the duration of the gaze shift (head contribution is shown, not the entire movement of the head during these trials). In each case, gaze shifts were initiated from the central target location and were made to the same target displaced down and left 35°. The eyes began in different horizontal positions relative to the head (Fig. 5B). One movement was initiated with the eyes deviated away from the horizontal J Neurophysiol • VOL

direction of the ensuing movement and in this case the eyes began deviated to the right (black). A second movement began with the eyes and head aligned (dark gray), and the third began with the eyes deviated to the left (in the direction of the movement: light gray). In each case, the eyes began in a central vertical position, and the vertical amplitudes of the eye movements were slightly ⬎30° (downward). Initial head positions (Fig. 5C) were opposite to those of the eyes. When the eyes began displaced to the right, the head began displaced (relative to the direction of gaze) deviated to the left (black). When the eyes began deviated to the left, the head began deviated to the right (light gray). In all three examples, the horizontal gaze shift amplitudes were nearly identical (37°) as were the vertical amplitudes (33°). When the eyes began deviated initially to the right (black) horizontal component amplitude was ⬎30°. Compare this to the 25° eye movement made when the eyes and head were aligned initially (dark gray) and 12.5° when the eyes began deviated to the left (light gray). Horizontal and vertical gaze (D), eye (E), and head (F) positions are plotted as functions of time. Inspection of the horizontal eye position traces from these three example trials (E) makes it clear that the

100 • DECEMBER 2008 •

www.jn.org

Downloaded from jn.physiology.org on August 31, 2010

Horizontal Eye Movement Duration (ms)

200

FIG. 3. Reversal of the relationship between saccade duration and amplitude during horizontal gaze shifts of similar amplitudes. Repeated gaze shifts to a target 35° left of fixation made with the eyes in different initial orbital positions. Data are from subject P (A) and subject S (E). Horizontal movement duration plotted as a function of saccade amplitude. Slope of lines of best fit, number of trials, and variance are shown in the legend. Three groups (n ⫽ 5) of trials are highlighted (gray filled symbols in A), and in panels B–D, for these trials gaze (black), eye (dark gray) and head (light gray) velocities are plotted as functions of time.

COUPLING DURING OBLIQUE GAZE SHIFTS

Horizontal components of oblique movements

A 200 160 120 25 80

subject P

20 25 35 40 50

50 50

10 20 30 40 50 Horizontal Eye Movement Amplitude (deg) r2 0.71 0.39 0.47 0.23 0.75

slope 0.8 -2.4 -3.1 -3.9 -4.7

N 274 221 514 139 53

20 30 35 40 50

r2

slope

0.57 0.41 0.46 0.38 0.51

0.3 -3.2 -3.0 -2.5 -2.8

N 314 51 299 58 70

FIG. 4. A: lines of best fit for duration-amplitude data for subjects P (black) and S (gray). Movements made to targets along the horizontal meridian. Target displacements indicated for each line. Note negative slopes for movements to all targets except target located 20° away from fixation location. In tabular form, B provides r2, slope and N for each line of best fit shown in A.

duration of the horizontal component of the movement initiated with the eyes deviated to the right (black)—the saccade with the largest horizontal component amplitude— had a shorter duration than did the movement initiated with the eyes deviated to the left (light gray). And although the amplitudes were the same, the duration of the vertical saccade component associated with the smallest amplitude horizontal component had the longest duration of these three movements. To complete the overview of these oblique gaze shifts, horizontal and vertical velocities are plotted as functions of time in G–I. Compared with the movements made when the eyes were deviated either to the right or centered in the orbits, there is a clear reduction in peak velocity and increase in duration of the vertical eye movement component during the movement associated with the smallest horizontal component (light gray). Several observations are illustrated in the examples shown in Fig. 5. First, as it was during movement along the horizontal meridian, there is a reversal of the duration-amplitude relationship for the horizontal component of eye movements that are part of constant amplitude and direction gaze shifts. Second, the duration of the vertical component of eye movements increases along with the duration of the horizontal component. This occurs despite the progressively smaller horizontal component amplitude coupled with constant amplitude vertical movements. Third, the curvature of saccades and gaze shifts appears to be correlated with the contribution of the head. In the figures that follow, details of each of these observations will be provided. J Neurophysiol • VOL

During movements along the horizontal meridian, the linear relationship between saccade duration and amplitude had a negative slope (Figs. 3 and 4). Figure 6 shows the relationship between duration and amplitude of horizontal saccade components of oblique gaze shifts. Movements were made to a target displaced 35° to the left and 35° down. In A, saccade duration is plotted as a function of amplitude for 214 movements made by subject P. The duration-amplitude relationship for similar movements to the same target (subject S) is plotted in B. In each case, the slopes of the lines of best fit are negative. This provides an opportunity to assess separately the effects of amplitude and duration of the horizontal saccade component on the duration of the constant amplitude vertical saccade component. As stated in the preceding text, the initial fixation location was always a target positioned straight ahead of the subject (0,0). Over the course of many gaze shifts to the same target, gaze shifts had very similar horizontal and vertical amplitudes. However, due to the head-alignment requirements of the task, the contribution of the head to the horizontal component of movements, and similarly the horizontal amplitude of saccadic eye movements varied. Recall that current hypotheses of the coupling between vertical and horizontal components generally rely on a calculation of movement direction to scale the velocities of the two components. Thus these models of the head-restrained saccadic system predict a positive correlation between the duration of the constant amplitude component and the varying amplitude of the orthogonal component. None of these models predict that the duration of the vertical component will be unrelated to the amplitude of the horizontal component nor do they predict that the relationship will have a negative slope. During repeated gaze shifts to a target 35° to the left and 35° down (Fig. 7), with the eyes in different initial horizontal positions, vertical saccade amplitudes were relatively constant (Fig. 7A: mean ⫾ SD vertical saccade amplitude ⫽ ⫺33.97 ⫾ 1.3°; B: ⫽ ⫺33.58 ⫾ 1.5°). The amplitudes of the horizontal saccade components varied from 10 to 38°. When the horizontal saccade amplitude was ⬃10°, the vertical component amplitude was about three times larger than the horizontal. During other movements (when the eyes began deviated away from the direction of movement), the horizontal and vertical components had approximately equal amplitudes. Figure 7 plots the duration of the vertical saccade component as a function of the amplitude of the horizontal component for subjects P (A) and S (B). In each case, the duration of the 34° vertical saccades was longer (⬃175 ms) when coupled with a small (10°) horizontal saccade component. In contrast, when horizontal and vertical components were of equal amplitude, the duration of the vertical component was ⬎50 ms shorter. Although the amplitude of the vertical saccade component was not changing, the duration was increased by 50 ms when the vertical component was three times larger than the horizontal component amplitude. Note in Fig. 7, A and B, the mean (⫾SD) duration of pure vertical saccades to a target 35° down is indicated along the ordinate (Fig. 6A: mean ⫽ 72.6 ms; B: mean ⫽ 77.5 ms). For comparison, in Fig. 7B, data from a second target (down 35° and to the right 35°) are shown (gray filled symbols). It is clear that during movements to this target the range of horizontal eye movements was significantly smaller (⬍10°) than

100 • DECEMBER 2008 •

www.jn.org

Downloaded from jn.physiology.org on August 31, 2010

B

30

20

40 0 0

40 40

35

subject S

Horizontal Eye Movement Duration (ms)

3381

3382

E. G. FREEDMAN

GAZE 0

-20

20

-40

-10

-20

-30

E -10

-10

-20 Horizontal

-10

Eye Position (deg)

0

-10

-20

-20

-30

-30

F

100

200

300

400

500

0

-30/0 -10 Vertical

-20

-40

600

300

H

0

100

200

300

400

-200

300

I

-300 Vertical -500 0 -200 -400

-400

Horizontal

-30 -40 Vertical

-50/0

-20

600

0

100

200

300

400

500

600

300 Horizontal

Head Velocity (deg/s)

-500 0

Eye Velocity (deg/s)

Gaze Velocity (deg/s)

500

0

Vertical

-20

-10

Horizontal

-300

-10

Horizontal

0

0

-300 Vertical 0 -200 -400

-600

-600 0

10

Horizontal

-30 0

20

Horizontal Position (deg)

-20

Vertical

0

-20

100

200

300

400

500

600

0

100

200

300

400

500

600

TIME (ms)

TIME (ms)

0

100

200

300

400

500

600

TIME (ms)

FIG. 5. Single trial examples illustrating 3 oblique movements made with the eyes in different orbital positions. In A, vertical gaze position is plotted as a function of horizontal position. All gaze shifts begin from the central fixation target (0,0). In B, vertical eye positions are plotted as functions of horizontal positions for the same 3 movements. The eyes began in different orbital positions: centered (dark gray), deviated to the right (black), and deviated to the left (light gray). C: vertical as a function of horizontal head positions from movement beginning until the end of the gaze shifts (head contribution) for the same 3 trials. Trials are taken from data shown in Fig. 6A. Horizontal and vertical gaze positions plotted as functions of time (D), and horizontal and vertical gaze velocities plotted as functions of time (G) are shown for comparison. Eye position (E) and velocity (H) traces, as well as head position (F) and velocity (I) traces are also shown.

during movements made to the target located down and to the left 35° (black symbols). These data reflect an asymmetry in the amplitude of head contributions during movements to some targets. Head contributions varied over a wide range for movements to the target 35° to the left but varied over a much smaller range during symmetrical movements to the right. Over the range where they can be compared, the data from the two targets were quite similar; however, the variance in vertical movement duration accounted for by horizontal eye-movement amplitudes during this set of rightward movements was much less than that accounted for during movements to the target down and left (see text following text and Table 1). During repeated gaze shifts to the same target, gaze amplitude and direction were relatively constant as were the ampliJ Neurophysiol • VOL

tudes of the vertical components of the saccadic eye movements. The relationship between vertical saccade duration and horizontal saccade amplitude could have a negative slope as shown in Fig. 7. In contrast, there was a positive correlation between vertical component duration and horizontal component duration. Figure 8 plots vertical duration as a function of horizontal duration for the same movements shown in Fig. 7. As shown, the lines of best fit to these data had positive slopes and horizontal component duration accounted for 73% (A) and 94% (B) of the variance in vertical saccade duration. Although the duration of small-amplitude horizontal saccades was longer than the duration of larger-amplitude movements, the vertical component duration was stretched in time so that its duration matched closely the duration of the horizontal component.

100 • DECEMBER 2008 •

www.jn.org

Downloaded from jn.physiology.org on August 31, 2010

Gaze Position (deg)

0

-30

G

-40

-10

10

10

-20

20

Horizontal Position (deg)

D

-40

0

-20

Horizontal Position (deg)

-30/0

HEAD

C

Head Position (deg)

-40

EYE

B Vertical Position (deg)

A

COUPLING DURING OBLIQUE GAZE SHIFTS

Subject P

150

100

Slope = -2.6 n = 161 Subject S

150

A

100

Oblique Gaze Shifts

200

50

0

5

10

15

20

25

30

35

40

Horizontal Eye Movement Amplitude (deg) FIG.

6. Oblique gaze shifts to a target 35° left and 35° down relative to the fixation target. Horizontal saccade component duration is plotted as a function of horizontal saccade component amplitude for 214 movements made by subject P (A) and 161 by subject S (B). Lines of best fit shown in each panel. U in A are the 3 movements presented in Fig. 5.

There was clearly temporal stretching of the vertical component of oblique saccades made under the conditions of this experiment. This is demonstrated in Figs. 7 and 8, each of which show that although the vertical amplitude was not changing, the duration varied from 75 to nearly 200 ms. However, the vertical component duration was longest when it was coupled with the horizontal saccade components having the smallest amplitudes (Fig. 7). This stretching of the ⬃34° vertical component by a horizontal component that was 1/3 the size arises from the prolonged duration of these small-amplitude horizontal components. Component stretching appears to be based not on the relative amplitudes of the horizontal and vertical saccade components, but rather on increasing the duration of the “shorter” component so that it matches closely the duration of the “longer” component. For each subject, data of this type were collected during gaze shifts to 16 oblique target locations (see METHODS). The horizontal (Xpos) and vertical (Ypos) locations of the targets, the number of gaze shifts made to that target and meeting the selection criteria (N), and the variance (r2) of the line of best fit for the relationship between vertical saccade duration and horizontal saccade amplitude are given in the first two columns of Table 1. For each subject, data are rank ordered from highest to lowest r2. Bold type indicates the data illustrated in Fig. 7. The third column provides the slope of the line of best fit and the 95% confidence interval on the slope. The values of r2 for the vertical saccade duration versus horizontal duration data (Fig. J Neurophysiol • VOL

Vertical Eye Movement Duration (ms)

Horizontal Eye Movement Duration (ms)

B 200

Slope = -2.0

150 n2= 214

r = 0.33 Subject P

100 Mean (SD) Duration of Pure Vertical -35° Saccades 72.6ms (10.6)

50

B

200

Vertical Eye Movement Duration (ms)

Horizontal Eye Movement Duration (ms)

Slope = -2.0 n = 214

Slope = -2.96

150 n2= 161

r = 0.39 Subject S

100 Mean (SD) Duration of Pure Vertical -35° Saccades 77.5ms (7.2)

5

10 15 20 25 30 35 Horizontal Eye Movement Amplitude (deg)

40

FIG. 7. For the same set of oblique movements shown in Fig. 6, the duration of the constant amplitude vertical saccade component is plotted as a function of the amplitude of the horizontal saccade component. Lines of best fit, slopes, number of trials, and variance are shown in each panel. For comparison, the mean (⫾SD) duration of pure vertical saccades (made when the head was unrestrained) to a target displaced 35° downward is shown along the ordinate in each panel. U in A are the 3 movements presented in Fig. 5. In B, data from subject S are presented. U in B plot vertical eye movement duration as a function horizontal eye movement amplitude for 209 movements made to the target located 35° down and 35° to the right. See also related text and Table 1.

100 • DECEMBER 2008 •

www.jn.org

Downloaded from jn.physiology.org on August 31, 2010

8) and the slopes and 95% confidence intervals are shown in columns 4 and 5 of the table. As shown, the slopes of the lines of best fit for the duration-amplitude data were statistically different from 0 for movements to 23 of the 32 target-subject pairs. For all cases in which statistical significance was reached, the slopes of the duration-amplitude data were negative. It should be noted that existing hypotheses designed to account for component stretching predict a tight positive correlation between vertical saccade duration and horizontal saccade amplitude. The data from the 9/32 target-subject pairs for which the 95% confidence intervals on the slopes included 0 are not consistent with the predictions of these hypotheses; there was no statistically significant relationship between vertical duration and horizontal amplitude during movements to these targets. In stark contrast to the amplitude-duration data, across all 16 oblique target locations, horizontal saccade duration accounted for much of the variance in the duration of the vertical component [mean variance accounted for: 78.5% (S), and 71.0% (P)]. The slopes of the duration-duration relationships were all positive, and all were statistically different from 0 (Table 1).

Oblique Gaze Shifts

A200

3383

3384 TABLE

E. G. FREEDMAN

1. Duration-amplitude and duration-duration relationships during oblique gaze shifts

Target Location (Xpos, Ypos)(n)

r2 Vertical Duration vs. Horizontal Amplitude

Slope (⫾ 95%)

r2 Vertical Duration vs. Horizontal Duration

Slope (⫾ 95%)

0.93 0.95 0.82 0.94 0.85 0.81 0.74 0.80 0.72 0.51 0.57 0.96 0.61 0.77 0.78 0.86

1.00 (0.15)* 0.94 (0.04)* 0.95 (0.08)* 0.99 (0.06)* 0.92 (0.06)* 0.90 (0.05)* 0.76 (0.12)* 0.81 (0.04)* 1.02 (0.08)* 0.49 (0.06)* 0.82 (0.10)* 1.07 (0.03)* 0.85 (0.07)* 0.76 (0.06)* 0.79 (0.08)* 1.01 (0.06)*

0.98 0.57 0.76 0.73 0.63 0.66 0.93 0.82 0.83 0.47 0.77 0.59 0.79 0.88 0.56 0.84

0.98 (0.04)* 0.82 (0.24)* 1.02 (0.09)* 0.93 (0.08)* 0.59 (0.13)* 0.76 (0.10)* 0.85 (0.04)* 0.81 (0.09)* 1.01 (0.06)* 0.66 (0.09)* 0.78 (0.10)* 0.67 (0.07)* 0.84 (0.06)* 0.79 (0.07)* 0.64 (0.08)* 0.67 (0.05)*

A. Subject S (⫺20, ⫺35) (220) (ⴚ35, ⴚ35) (161) (⫺35, ⫺20) (132) (⫺20, ⫺20) (167) (⫺20, 20) (174) (⫺35, 35) (264) (⫺35, 20) (131) (20, 20) (385) (35, ⫺20) (267) (⫺20, 35) (312) (20, 35) (269) (20, ⫺35) (255) (20, ⫺20) (338) (35, 35) (175) (35, 20) (125) (35, ⫺35) (209)

⫺4.30 (1.85)* ⴚ2.96 (0.58)* ⫺2.60 (0.62)* ⫺4.90 (1.22)* ⫺3.40 (1.10)* ⫺1.60 (0.40)* ⫺1.30 (0.48)* ⫺1.12 (0.43)* ⫺1.52 (1.32)* ⫺0.58 (0.46)* 0.55 (0.57) 0.80 (1.12) 1.26 (2.18) 0.25 (0.62) ⫺0.35 (0.93) ⫺0.20 (1.73)

0.55 0.39 0.33 0.27 0.24 0.18 0.15 0.07 0.02 0.02 0.014 0.008 0.006 0.004 0.004 0.000

(⫺20, ⫺35) (39) (⫺35, 20) (101) (⫺35, ⫺20) (139) (ⴚ35, ⴚ35) (214) (⫺20, 35) (54) (35, ⫺20) (135) (⫺20, ⫺20) (124) (35, 20) (135) (⫺35, 35) (205) (35, 35) (209) (20, ⫺35) (76) (20, 20) (77) (⫺20, 20) (269) (20, ⫺20) (70) (20, 35) (202) (35, ⫺35) (168)

⫺3.70 (0.80)* ⫺2.59 (0.60)* ⫺2.67 (0.57)* ⴚ2.00 (0.42)* ⫺3.00 (1.30)* ⫺2.80 (0.90)* ⫺2.00 (0.70)* ⫺2.60 (1.00)* ⫺1.60 (0.56)* ⫺1.63 (0.77)* ⫺1.09 (0.95)* ⫺1.54 (0.74)* ⫺1.17 (0.60)* ⫺0.79 (1.51) 0.38 (1.10) 0.08 (0.81)

0.66 0.44 0.40 0.33 0.30 0.23 0.20 0.18 0.14 0.07 0.06 0.06 0.05 0.02 0.002 0.000

For subject S and P, horizontal and vertical locations of the target (Xpos, Ypos) and number of trials (n) are presented in the leftmost column. Vertical saccade duration was plotted as a function of horizontal saccade amplitude, and lines were fit using least-squares methods. The variance in vertical duration accounted for by changes in horizontal amplitude (r2) is shown in column 2. The slope of each line of best fit and the 95% confidence interval on the slopes are shown in column 3. Column 4 shows the r2 values for the duration-duration data, and column 5 presents the slopes and 95% confidence intervals on these slopes. *, slopes significantly different from 0. Bold text indicates data illustrated in Figs. 6 and 7. Bold gray text indicates data shown using gray-filled symbols in Fig. 6B. Data are ordered from highest to lowest duration-amplitude r2 for each subject.

Because the horizontal and vertical components of gaze shifts were similar for these movements, to achieve the reversal in the duration-amplitude relationship for the horizontal saccade components, movements had to be initiated with the eyes in a variety of orbital positions. One might think that the increased duration of the horizontal component seen when the eyes begin deviated in the direction of the ensuing movement (e.g., light gray in Fig. 5) is a result of orbital mechanical effects and not due to interactions between the saccade and the ongoing head movement. To allay these concerns, head-restrained movements were analyzed. During 3,117 headrestrained saccades made to the same 24 target locations (16 oblique, 4 along the horizontal meridian and 4 along the vertical) used during the head-unrestrained trials and initiated from the same orbital eye positions, 137 successful movements were made to the target located 35° to the left and 35° down relative to the central fixation location. These conditions are identical to those illustrated in Fig. 7 except that in this case the head was prevented from moving. Figure 9 plots the durations of the vertical eye movements as a function of the amplitudes of the horizontal saccade J Neurophysiol • VOL

components. When the eyes began centered in the orbits, horizontal movement amplitudes were on the order of 32°. However, when the eyes began deviated away from the direction of movement and the head remained restrained and pointing straight ahead, saccade amplitudes increased to ⬃48°. When the eyes began deviated in the direction of movement, saccade amplitudes were ⬃15°. In the headrestrained case as shown here, the changes in movement amplitudes are consistent with the altered displacements required to bring the image of the target onto the fovea. With the head restrained at zero, when the eyes begin deviated 15° to the right, a gaze shift of ⬃50° is required to look to the target 35° to the left. The durations of the constant vertical components varied as saccade amplitudes varied. As shown there is a positive correlation between the duration of the vertical saccade and the amplitude of the horizontal components of these oblique saccades. The reversal of the duration-amplitude relationship seen in Fig. 7 depends on the varying movements of the head associated with each gaze shift and not on the positions of the eyes in the orbits. When the head does not move, vertical duration

100 • DECEMBER 2008 •

www.jn.org

Downloaded from jn.physiology.org on August 31, 2010

B. Subject P

COUPLING DURING OBLIQUE GAZE SHIFTS

Oblique Gaze Shifts (Horiz. = -35 ; Vert. = -35 ) 200 Slope = 0.93 n = 214 150 r 2 = 0.73 Subject P

100 Vertical Eye Amplitude (mean ± SD) = -33.97° ± 1.3

50

200 Slope = 0.95 n = 161 150 r 2 = 0.94 Subject P

100 Vertical Eye Amplitude (mean ± SD) = -33.58° ± 1.5

50

100 150 Horizontal Eye Movement Duration (ms)

200

FIG. 8. For the same trials illustrated in Figs. 6 and 7, vertical saccade duration is plotted as a function of horizontal saccade duration. Lines of best fit, slopes, number of trials, and variance are shown in each panel. Mean (⫾SD) amplitude of the vertical saccade components are shown in each panel.

and horizontal amplitude are tightly coupled and positively correlated. Straightness of oblique gaze shifts Straightness is a well-documented characteristic of headrestrained saccades. Straight saccades are produced when horizontal and vertical components begin and end together and when their velocity profiles differ only by a scaling factor (see Fig. 1 and related text). Figure 10A illustrates gaze (black), eye (dark gray, and head contribution (light gray) trajectories for a set of movements made to a target displaced leftward 35° and downward 35°. Total head-movement amplitudes were between 35 and 50° during these movements, all traces are aligned at zero. As is clear from the eye and gaze trajectories, movements were dramatically curved. Head movements during another set of trials to the same target location were much smaller (between 0 and 15°), and gaze, eye and head contribution trajectories for this set of movements are illustrated in Fig. 10B. Note that the amplitudes of the horizontal and vertical components of these gaze shifts were nearly identical to movements shown in A. Despite the similarities in gaze shift amplitude and direction, these movements appear to be much straighter. This statement is reinforced by plotting vertical as a function of horizontal velocity and calculating the VAF for each trial. Velocity-velocity plots for the gaze (black) and eye J Neurophysiol • VOL

200

Head Restrained Oblique Saccadic Eye Movements Target Location (-35,-35)

150

Slope = 1.2 n = 137 r2 = 0.85 Subject P

100

50 0

Eyes deviated away from movement direction Eyes centered in orbits

Eyes deviated in the direction of movement

10

20

30

40

50

60

| Horizontal Eye Movement Amplitude (deg) | FIG. 9. Vertical duration is plotted as a function of horizontal amplitude for head-restrained saccades made with the eyes in orbital positions that match the positions of the eyes during the laser pointing task with the head unrestrained. Orbital position effects cannot account for the negative slope observed during head-unrestrained gaze shifts (Fig. 7).

100 • DECEMBER 2008 •

www.jn.org

Downloaded from jn.physiology.org on August 31, 2010

Vertical Eye Movement Duration (ms)

B

(eye) movements corresponding to trajectories plotted in Fig. 10A are shown in D. Horizontal velocity accounted for 45% of the variance in vertical velocity during these movements. During the movements shown in Fig. 10B, horizontal velocity accounted for 92% of the variance (D). During gaze shifts of the same amplitude and direction, gaze trajectory curvature depended on the amplitude of the associated head movements; gaze shift curvature is direction-independent. This direction independence of gaze shift curvature is demonstrated by comparing the curvature of gaze shifts (black) in A to those shown in B. The directions are identical, but curvature depends on the associated head movement. Similarly, the curvature of the eye movement component of gaze shifts can be shown to be direction-independent. Compare the eye movements in A (gray) with those shown in C. The movements shown in C were selected so that the amplitude and direction of the saccadic eye movements matched those made during the trials shown in A. Trajectories of these eye movements are shown along with the trajectories of the gaze shifts and associated head movements. Comparison of eye movements in A and C illustrates that curvature of eye movements does not depend on movement direction; it is, however, correlated with the contribution of the head. In F, vertical velocity is plotted as a function of horizontal velocity for these eye movements and as shown, horizontal velocity accounted for 89% of the variance in vertical velocity. As with gaze shifts, eye-movement curvature appears to be direction-independent and correlated with the contribution of the head. Table 2 summarizes the curvature data for gaze shifts and eye movements to 10 different target locations for subjects S and P. Target locations correspond to the locations given in Table 1 for each subject (the 1st 10 locations in order). Movements to each target were divided into groups having head movements larger than 35° (large head contribution in Table 2), and movements having head movements smaller than 15° (small). Mean r2 (SD) for gaze and eye movements are shown for each group. K-S tests were used to compare the variance during movements having large and small head contributions. In every case, gaze shifts and eye movements

Vertical Eye Movement Duration (ms)

Vertical Eye Movement Duration (ms)

A

3385

3386

E. G. FREEDMAN

B

A

C

Vertical Position (deg)

10 Head

0 -10 -20 Gaze -30

Eye

-40 -40

-20

0 -40

-20

0 -40

-20

0

Horizontal Position (deg)

F

Vertical Velocity (deg/s)

0

-200

Gaze

-400

Gaze

2

2

r = 0.92(0.03)

-600 Gaze -600

Eye

Eye

2

r = 0.45(0.14)

-400

2

0

Eye

2

r = 0.61(0.14)

-200

r = 0.89(0.04)

2

r = 0.93(0.02)

-600

-400

-200

r = 0.89(0.04)

0

-600

-400

-200

0

Horizontal Velocity (deg/s) FIG.

10. Curvature of oblique gaze shifts. A: head contribution (light gray), eye (dark gray), and gaze (black) trajectories plotted for a set of movements made to a target 35° down and 35° left. Trajectories are aligned at the origin for comparison. Gaze (black) and eye (gray) vertical velocities are plotted as functions of horizontal velocities (D) for movements shown in A. Head-movement amplitudes were larger than 35° during this set of trials. For each movement, r2 values were calculated and mean (⫾SD) r2 values are shown. Similar plots are shown for another set of movements to the same target location during which the head moved between 0 and 15° (B and E). Movements to a different target (⫺20,⫺35) and associated velocity-velocity traces are shown in C and F.

associated with large head movements were more curved (variance accounted for was statistically significantly lower: P ⬍ 0.05) than during matched movements associated with small head contributions. Curvature of both gaze shifts and eye movements depends on the amplitude of the associated head movement, not on the direction of movement. DISCUSSION

The horizontal and vertical components of oblique saccades made when the head is prevented from moving are not independent (Bahill et al. 1975; Baloh et al. 1975; Becker and Ju¨rgens 1990; Collewijn et al. 1988a,b; Evinger et al. 1981; Fuchs 1967; Guitton and Mandl 1980; King et al. 1986; Smit et al. 1990; van Gisbergen et al. 1985; Yee et al. 1985). Several mechanisms for the coupling of components have been proposed (Becker and Ju¨rgens 1990; Grossman and Robinson 1988; Nichols and Sparks 1996; Quaia and Optican 1997; Smit et al. 1990; van Gisbergen et al. 1985; van Opstal and Kappen J Neurophysiol • VOL

1993). Extending these hypotheses to include coordinated movements of the eyes and head has yet to be adequately addressed. By systematically manipulating the positions of the eyes in the orbits at the onset of constant amplitude and direction gaze shifts, it is possible to probe the nature of the coupling of horizontal and vertical saccade components in ways that are not possible when the head is prevented from moving. Using this approach several key points have been demonstrated in this report. First, during gaze shifts to the same target location along the horizontal meridian, when the initial positions of the eyes in the orbits vary, the relationship between saccade duration and amplitude has a negative slope. This observation has not been made explicitly before; however, it is a direct consequence of two aspects of eye-head coordination both of which have been previously described. The first is that during constant amplitude gaze shifts the amplitudes of the eye and head components of movements vary inversely; small

100 • DECEMBER 2008 •

www.jn.org

Downloaded from jn.physiology.org on August 31, 2010

E

D

COUPLING DURING OBLIQUE GAZE SHIFTS

Eye r2 Head Contribution

Large

Large

Small

0.37 ⫾ 0.14* 0.44 ⫾ 0.22* 0.37 ⫾ 0.12* 0.41 ⫾ 0.12* 0.58 ⫾ 0.16* 0.73 ⫾ 0.12* 0.65 ⫾ 0.11* 0.74 ⫾ 0.09* 0.71 ⫾ 0.10* 0.59 ⫾ 0.07*

0.93 ⫾ 0.05 0.88 ⫾ 0.08 0.82 ⫾ 0.12 0.89 ⫾ 0.12 0.90 ⫾ 0.05 0.95 ⫾ 0.03 0.92 ⫾ 0.06 0.95 ⫾ 0.03 0.96 ⫾ 0.02 0.97 ⫾ 0.01

Small A. Subject S

⫾ ⫾ ⫾ ⫾ ⫾ ⫾ ⫾ ⫾ ⫾ ⫾

0.18* 0.25* 0.12* 0.14* 0.17* 0.10* 0.10* 0.09* 0.13* 0.07*

0.93 ⫾ 0.03 0.85 ⫾ 0.08 0.84 ⫾ 0.12 0.88 ⫾ 0.13 0.89 ⫾ 0.06 0.95 ⫾ 0.03 0.91 ⫾ 0.07 0.96 ⫾ 0.02 0.98 ⫾ 0.01 0.97 ⫾ 0.01

0.45 0.45 0.61 0.53 0.56 0.51 0.55 0.74 0.64 0.60

⫾ ⴞ ⫾ ⫾ ⫾ ⫾ ⫾ ⫾ ⫾ ⫾

0.16* 0.14* 0.04* 0.11* 0.15* 0.13* 0.09* 0.11* 0.12* 0.10*

0.84 ⫾ 0.17 0.92 ⴞ 0.03 0.89 ⴞ 0.04 0.95 ⫾ 0.03 0.96 ⫾ 0.01 0.98 ⫾ 0.01 0.94 ⫾ 0.04 0.95 ⫾ 0.04 0.97 ⫾ 0.01 0.95 ⫾ 0.03

B. Subject P 0.48 ⫾ 0.17* 0.61 ⴞ 0.14* 0.67 ⫾ 0.03* 0.58 ⫾ 0.12* 0.61 ⫾ 0.18* 0.66 ⫾ 0.16* 0.63 ⫾ 0.08* 0.75 ⫾ 0.12* 0.69 ⫾ 0.11* 0.64 ⫾ 0.14*

0.88 ⫾ 0.08 0.93 ⴞ 0.02 0.89 ⴞ 0.04 0.95 ⫾ 0.02 0.95 ⫾ 0.01 0.97 ⫾ 0.01 0.95 ⫾ 0.03 0.94 ⫾ 0.04 0.97 ⫾ 0.01 0.95 ⫾ 0.04

For subjects S and P, the table provides mean r2 (⫾SD) values calculated from plots of vertical versus horizontal velocity for gaze shifts (2 columns on left) and eye movements (2 columns on right). Data are from the first 10 target locations listed in Table 1 (chosen because of the large range of head movements made during these trials). Movements having associated head movements ⬎35° are in the “large head contribution” columns. Movements having small head movements (0 –15°) are shown in the “small head contribution” columns. Bold entries indicate data from Fig. 11, A and B. Bold italicized entry shows data from Fig. 11, C. r2 values during large head movements were all statistically smaller than corresponding r2 values during small head movements (Kolmogorov-Smimoff tests P ⬍ 0.05).

saccadic eye movements are coupled with large (and fast) head movements, whereas large saccades are coupled with small head movements (Freedman 2005; Freedman and Sparks 1997a,b; Stahl 1999, 2001; Tomlinson 1990). The second important observation is that as the amplitude and speed of head movements increase, the velocity of the eyes declines (Freedman and Sparks 2000). During gaze shifts initiated with the eyes deviated away from the direction of movement, the head contribution and speed of the head will be small, and the large concomitant saccade will have a high peak velocity. However, when the eyes are deviated in the direction of the ensuing movements, a fast (and large) head movement will be associated with a small-amplitude saccade that has a reduced velocity. This results in the reversed duration-amplitude relationship seen during constant amplitude gaze shifts initiated with the eyes in different initial positions (Figs. 3 and 4). During oblique gaze shifts of similar amplitude and direction, when the eyes begin in the same vertical positions but in different horizontal positions, the amplitude of the vertical saccade component was relatively constant, but the amplitude of the horizontal saccade component varied. Under these conditions, it was possible to assess the effects of varying horizontal component amplitude on the duration of the constant amplitude vertical saccade component. In addition, under these conditions, it was possible to test the assumption that the J Neurophysiol • VOL

400 Eye Head 0

120

-400

100

80 15

25

35

Amplitude (deg)

-800 0

100

200

300 TIME (ms)

400

500

FIG. 11. Eye (thick lines) and head (thin lines) velocities are plotted as functions of time for 3 movements. Movements selected from data presented in Fig. 7. The time at which peak eye velocity occurs is marked (dashed vertical lines). Inset: the negative slope of the duration-amplitude relationship for these 3 example trials. See text for discussion of the role of the VOR during gaze shifts of this type.

100 • DECEMBER 2008 •

www.jn.org

Downloaded from jn.physiology.org on August 31, 2010

0.37 0.40 0.38 0.36 0.59 0.70 0.54 0.75 0.70 0.65

Duration (ms)

Gaze r2 Head Contribution

constant amplitude vertical component is temporally stretched as a function of the amplitude of the horizontal component. As shown here, the relative amplitudes of horizontal and vertical components were not a good predictor of the duration of the vertical component. Instead the duration of the horizontal component (regardless of amplitude) appears to determine the duration of the vertical component. This occurred even when the vertical component was three times larger than the amplitude of the horizontal component. During coordinated eye-head movements, the eyes and head move in the same direction. The operation of the vestibuloocular reflex (VOR) during these movements might be expected to reduce the velocity of the eyes in proportion to the ongoing velocity of the head. This assumes that the VOR remains fully functional during eye-head movements. The role of the VOR during gaze shifts remains unclear with data from a number of experiments producing a variety of conclusions including complete absence of the VOR during gaze shifts, modulation of VOR along a time course related to gaze motor error, and no modulation of VOR during gaze shifts (Cullen et al. 2004; Dichgans et al. 1974; Guitton and Volle 1987; Guitton et al. 1984; Laurutis and Robinson 1986; Pelisson and Prablanc 1986; Tabak et al. 1996; Viirre et al. 1986). The pertinent issue is whether or not operation of the VOR could account for the data in this report. To address this concern, Fig. 11 plots eye (thick lines) and head (thin lines) velocities as functions of time for three example movements taken from data shown in Fig. 8A. As shown, one of these saccades was associated with a small (light gray: 4°), one with a medium (dark gray: 28°), and one with a large (black: 45°) head movement. Dashed lines indicate the time at which the early peak in eye velocity occurs for each trial. Head velocity for the smallest head movement is near zero at the time of peak saccade velocities. During the two movements with larger head movements, head velocity was ⬍30°/s at the time that peak eye velocities occurred. However, eye velocities differed by as much as 500°/s when comparing the movement with the largest and smallest head contributions. For the VOR to account for this dramatic decrease in saccade velocity, the gain of the reflex would need to exceed 16. Note that this analysis of the VOR depends on the assumption that

Velocity (deg/s)

2. Curvature of gaze shifts and eye movements when head movements are large or small

TABLE

3387

3388

E. G. FREEDMAN

the gain of the VOR is similar during gaze shifts having the same amplitudes and directions. While the available data suggest that this is a reasonable assumption, this is an issue that requires further attention. In addition to the comparison in the preceding text, if the two movements with larger head contributions are considered, there is a difference in peak saccade velocity of ⬃250°/s, but at this same point in time, there was no difference in the velocities of the head. The reacceleration of the eyes seen in both of the movements associated with larger head contributions is also difficult to account for using a VOR-based mechanism. Whether the VOR remains partially or completely operational during large-amplitude gaze shifts remains an open question, but it is not adequate to account for the data presented here. Yet the reduced velocity of small-amplitude saccades seen during constant amplitude gaze shifts leads to the reversal of the saccadic duration-amplitude relationship (inset) and must be accounted for in any attempt to model the gaze control system.

The reversal of the saccadic duration-amplitude relationship observed during head unrestrained gaze shifts, and the impact that this has on the duration of the constant amplitude vertical component of oblique movements may be surprising. It might also seem that some of the core assumptions that have formed the basis for our understanding of the oculomotor system are more tenuous than previously thought. For instance, the hypothetical saccadic burst generator has been presumed to convert signals of dynamic motor error into a motor command proportional to saccade velocity (cf. Becker and Ju¨rgens 1990; Ju¨rgens et al. 1981; Robinson 1973; van Gisbergen et al. 1987;). This function (in conjunction with the feedback control loop) enables models of the saccadic system to reproduce the monotonic, saturating relationship between peak saccade velocity and movement amplitude as well as the linear, positive correlation between saccade duration and amplitude observed when the head is restrained. But the reversal of the duration-amplitude relationship raises questions about the nature of the burst generator and the exponential function that has been used to model the transformation of motor error into a velocity command. Hypotheses designed to capture the main features of the saccadic system assume that the observed amplitude of a saccade is a veridical representation of the input to the burst generator; models assume that the desired displacement of the eyes equals the amplitude of the observed movement. However, when the head contributes to the accomplishment of gaze shifts, multiple subsystems (e.g., saccadic system, vestibuloocular and cervicoocular reflexes) may all be contributing simultaneously to the movements of the eyes. The observed saccadic eye movement may no longer be a good predictor of the desired displacement signal that serves as the initial input to the saccadic burst generator. If this is the case, knowledge of the state of the saccadic, or in some models gaze, burst generator cannot be inferred from either the amplitude or from the temporal progression of the observed movements. As a result it could be the case that the reversal in the durationamplitude relationship during head-unrestrained gaze shifts does not reflect a dramatic change in the nature of the burst J Neurophysiol • VOL

Extending current models There are numerous gaze control models (e.g., Bizzi et al. 1971, 1972; Freedman 2001; Galiana and Guitton 1992; Goossens and van Opstal 1997; Guitton and Volle 1987; Guitton et al. 1990; Phillips et al. 1995; Tomlinson 1990; Tweed 1997; Volle and Guitton 1993). In general they differ in their assumptions about the order of linear and nonlinear elements such as the burst generator, the separation of gaze into eye and head commands, the decomposition of vectorial into horizontal and vertical component signals, and the position of these hypothetical elements within or outside of the dynamic feedback control loop. The vast majority of these hypotheses are concerned only with movement along the horizontal meridian. The means by which these models are extended to include movements in two dimensions will depend on assumptions about the uncoupling of horizontal and vertical components (is there a single vectorial burst generator or separate, interacting horizontal and vertical burst generators?), the position of this uncoupling (is there a vectorial gaze burst generator? Separate vectorial eye and head burst generators? Separate eye and head, horizontal and vertical burst generators? And how do these interact?). A detailed exposition of each of these possible hypotheses, its predictions, and its compatibility with existing

100 • DECEMBER 2008 •

www.jn.org

Downloaded from jn.physiology.org on August 31, 2010

Implications for neural control of gaze shifts

generator function but reveals an uncoupling of the saccade burst generator from the observed behavior. This “neural uncertainty” (Frens et al. 1998; Sparks 1999; Stanford and Sparks 1994) could prevent development of hypotheses based on the observed amplitude-duration-velocity relationships when the head is free to move. Without a clear understanding of all of the inputs that contribute to the movements of the eyes and head, and the ways in which they interact to produce the observed movements, it may not possible to generate useful hypotheses. In fact, to the extent that there is uncertainty about the state of the burst generator during eye-head movements, all existing gaze control models may be rendered moot. To progress, this conundrum must be circumvented. One solution is to understand and experimentally isolate all of the input signals to the burst generator, thereby determining its state during gaze shifts. Until this is accomplished, making the assumption (albeit a potentially problematic one) that the observed movements reflect the state of the burst generator permits continued progress toward understanding the neural control of coordinated eye-head movements. As yet untested, this assumption is not implausible; observed saccades may be good predictors of the state of the saccadic burst generator even during eye-head movements. For instance, tests of the gain of the VOR during large-amplitude movements indicate a significant reduction in reflex efficacy during gaze shifts, thus reducing any impact of this subsystem on observed movement amplitudes and kinematics. It is also possible that reflexes during active movements operate only on unexpected movements of the head and that as long as head movements progress according to the head motor command, the vestibular system does not alter ongoing saccades. Again if this was the case, the amplitude and kinematics of the observed saccade would indeed reflect the comparator and burst generator inputs. Given the possibility that the assumption is valid, it is worth considering what modifications to existing hypotheses are required to account for the results described in this report.

COUPLING DURING OBLIQUE GAZE SHIFTS

J Neurophysiol • VOL

Model Simulations Horizontal Gaze Shifts of Constant Amplitude (40 )

Horizontal Eye Position (deg)

A 25 20

0

-20 0

Horizontal Eye Velocity (deg/s)

B

500 Time (ms)

1000

500 Time (ms)

1000

600

400

200

0

-200 0

C 220 Horizontal Duration (ms)

FIG. 12. Simulations using the Freedman (2001) model. Horizontal eye positions (A) and velocities (B) are plotted as functions of time for 7 simulated gaze shifts having amplitudes of 40°. The (simulated) positions of the eyes in the orbits was varied from 25° in the direction opposite the movement (black) to 25° in the direction of movement (magenta). For simulated movements having gaze amplitudes of 40, 35, 30, and 20°, the duration-amplitude relationships for the eye component of these gaze shifts are illustrated (C). Compare these simulations to the data presented in Fig. 4.

reduces the gain of both the horizontal and vertical saccadic burst generators. Similarly a vertical head velocity signal is assumed to reduce the saccadic burst generator gains. Using a model having this basic design, it was possible to simulate

180

40 40 35 30 20

35

slope = -4.2 slope = -3.3 slope = -0.77 slope = 0.33

140 30

100 60 0

100 • DECEMBER 2008 •

20

10 20 30 Horizontal Amplitude (deg)

www.jn.org

40

Downloaded from jn.physiology.org on August 31, 2010

behavioral data is beyond the scope of this report. Instead the focus here will be to extend one hypothesis (Freedman 2001) and show that this model can account for the amplitudeduration reversal, and by extending the Becker and Ju¨rgens (1990) cross-coupling scheme to include the eye-head interaction mechanism described previously, it is possible to account for the data presented in this report. Simulations using the model first proposed by Freedman (2001) are shown in Fig. 12, A—C, and demonstrate the ability of this model to account for the reversal of the durationamplitude relationship observed during constant amplitude gaze shifts initiated with the eyes in different orbital positions. Note that with the exception of the two inputs (desired gaze displacement and initial eye position), nothing in the original model was changed to produce these simulations; all parameters were identical to those described in the published account. In A, simulated horizontal eye positions are plotted as functions of time for seven movements. In each case, desired gaze displacement was 40°. Eye position at the onset of movements ranged from 25° in the direction opposite to the movement direction (⫺25°) to 25° in the direction of the movement. When the eyes began deviated in the orbits ⫺25°, peak saccade velocity (B, black trace) was high and duration short. Saccade amplitude during this simulated movement was 37° and movement duration was ⬍90 ms (Fig. 12C, right-most black filled symbol). As the eyes began in more central positions, and then as the eyes began deviated in the direction of movements, eye-movement amplitudes declined. As shown in B, as initial eye positions changed, peak velocity declined and durations increased. The duration-amplitude data for these simulated horizontal gaze shifts having different amplitudes are shown in C. For each set of simulations, gaze shift amplitudes were constant. As simulated saccade amplitude declined, duration increased and the slopes of the lines of best fit were negative. Compare these simulations with the data in Fig. 4. An extension of the Freedman (2001) hypothesis to include movements in two dimensions is illustrated schematically in Fig. 13A. In this scheme, a vectorial desired displacement command is derived from the active population of motorrelated neurons in the superior colliculus. In the model, this command is decomposed into horizontal and vertical desired displacement commands (gray filled square). Then using information about the positions of the eyes in the orbits, horizontal and vertical gaze commands are separated into desired eye (horizontal and vertical) and desired head (horizontal and vertical) displacement signals (Freedman 2001, 2005). At this stage, the desired eye displacement signals serve as the input to a standard cross-coupled 2D model of saccadic eye movements (Becker and Ju¨rgens 1990). Finally, to account for the reduction in both horizontal and vertical saccade velocities as a function of increasing horizontal head contribution, in an extension of the Freedman (2001) gaze control model, it is suggested that a signal proportional to horizontal head velocity

3389

3390

E. G. FREEDMAN

A

1/s Vert. Des. Eye Disp. Horizontal Gaze Desired Displacement Horiz. Eye Position

Vectorial Gaze Desired Displacement

Vertical Eye BG Cross-Coupling Mechanism

Horiz. Des. Eye Disp.

Vert. Eye Velocity

-

Gain

Horizontal Eye BG

Horiz. Eye Velocity

Vert. Eye Position

Horiz. Des. Horizontal Head Disp. Head BG

Horiz. Head Velocity

Vertical Gaze Desired Displacement Vert. Des. Vertical Head Head Disp. BG Vert. Head Velocity

B

C 200 Vertical Eye Duration (ms)

Vertical Eye Duration (ms)

200

150

150

100

100 Slope = -2.7 50

0

Slope = 1.0

10 20 30 40 Horizontal Eye Amplitude (deg)

50

140 180 100 Horizontal Eye Duration (ms)

oblique gaze shifts of constant amplitude (40°) and direction (45° above the horizontal) with the eyes in different horizontal initial positions. In Fig. 14B, vertical eye movement duration is plotted as a function of horizontal saccade amplitude for simulations using a Becker and Ju¨rgens cross-coupling factor of 0.1. As illustrated the slope of the linear correlation of these data were negative (⫺2.7: compare with data in Fig. 7). For the same simulations, the durations of the horizontal and vertical components of saccades were positively correlated (Fig. 14C; slope ⫽ 1.0; compare with data in Fig. 8). As shown, the eye-head interaction suggested by Freedman (2001) reduces the gain of the horizontal burst generator as a function of the head velocity command. This enables this model to account for the reversal of the duration-amplitude relationship. There are other methods that might also reduce the gain of the horizontal burst generator. For instance, partial inactivation of neurons if the PPRF might accomplish the same thing, even if the head were restrained. Barton and colleagues (2003) have used this approach to assess component stretching during oblique headrestrained saccades. Saccades became increasingly curved after PPRF inactivation, and this implies that movements of the same amplitude and direction took longer to complete. Comparison of the duration-amplitude relationships after lidocaine injection does not provide clear evidence for a reversal in the expected headrestrained relationship. It is difficult to determine whether these J Neurophysiol • VOL

results are similar to those reported here. And it should be mentioned that lidocaine injections in the PPRF may not reduce the gain of the saccadic burst generator without also altering other related functions. It remains to be shown that burst generator gain reductions caused by other mechanisms will result in similar duration-amplitude relationship reversals. In addition to being compatible with the reversal of the slope of the duration-amplitude relationship during horizontal gaze shifts, and accounting for the effects on both horizontal and vertical component duration during oblique movements, the model presented schematically in Fig. 13, can also account for the direction-independent curvature of gaze and eye movements observed during gaze shifts. In Fig. 14, vertical gaze (A) and eye (B) positions are plotted as functions of horizontal gaze and eye position during three simulated movements. The eyes and head began aligned (black) or the eyes began deviated in the direction of the gaze shift (light gray) or in the opposite direction (dark gray). Vertical and horizontal velocities for gaze shifts (D) and the eye-movement components of gaze (E) are shown plotted as functions of time. Note as head contribution increases, there are dramatic changes in horizontal velocity profiles of these simulated gaze shifts. To assess movement curvature, vertical velocity is plotted as a function of horizontal velocity for the gaze shift with the largest (G) and smallest (H) head contribution. When head contribution was small, 99% of

100 • DECEMBER 2008 •

www.jn.org

Downloaded from jn.physiology.org on August 31, 2010

FIG. 13. Schematic representation of an extension of the Freedman (2001) gaze control model designed to account for movements in 2 dimensions (A). Cross-coupling of horizontal and vertical eye movements (dashed lines) is accomplished using the model of Becker and Ju¨rgens (1990) with a coupling constant of 0.1. Using this model, simulations of gaze shifts to a target displaced 40° horizontally and 40° vertically with the eyes in different orbital positions were carried out. The duration of the vertical saccade component is plotted as a function of the amplitude of the horizontal saccade (B). Note the negative slope of this linear relationship. Vertical component duration is plotted as a function of horizontal component duration (C). Slopes of the lines of best fit are provided in each panel. Compare these simulations to the data in Figs. 6 and 7.

1/s

COUPLING DURING OBLIQUE GAZE SHIFTS

Vertical Position (deg)

40 35 30 25 20 15 10 5

A

3391

B

C

Gaze

Eye - Matched Amplitudes and Directions

Eye

Large Head Contrib.

Small Head Contribution 0

10

20

30

-20

40

-10

0

10

20

40 0

30

10

20

30

40

Horizontal Position (deg)

D 600

E

Gaze

F

Eye

Eye - Matched Amplitudes and Directions

Velocity (deg/s)

Vertical

Vertical

Vertical

Horizontal

Horizontal

600/0

300

Horizontal

0

0

200

0

200

0

200

Vertical Velocity (deg/s)

Time (ms) G

H

Eye r2 = 0.62

I

600 Eye - Matched Amplitudes and Directions

600 Small Head Contribution

400 Large Head Contribution

r2 = 0.99 Eye r2 = 0.99

Gaze r2 = 0.59

Gaze r2 = 0.99

r2 = 0.83 r2 = 0.64

0 0

300

0

600

0

600

Horizontal Velocity (deg/s) FIG.

14. Simulations illustrating the head-contribution dependence of gaze and eye-movement curvature captured by the hypothesis outlined. A: trajectories of 3 simulated gaze shifts to a target 35° up and 35° right. The initial eye positions were varied during these simulations; eye-movement trajectories are shown in B. Vertical and horizontal gaze (D) and eye (E) velocities as functions time are also shown. When simulated head movements were large (light gray), gaze shift and eye movements were significantly more curved than when head movements were small (dark gray). G and H plot vertical as a function of horizontal velocity and show eye and gaze r2 values. C plots eye-movement trajectories during 3 simulated movements that have eye movements of the same amplitude and direction. Head-movement amplitudes varied: 0° (black), 25° (dark gray), and 40° (light gray). Vertical and horizontal eye velocities (F) are plotted as functions of time for these 3 simulations. I: vertical as a function of horizontal velocity and provides r2 values for each.

the variance was accounted for by horizontal velocity for both gaze shift and associated eye movements (H); movements were straight (dark gray in A and B). In contrast, during gaze shifts of the same amplitude and direction, both eye and gaze shift curvature was much larger when the head contribution was J Neurophysiol • VOL

large (G). These simulations demonstrate the ability of the model outlined in Fig. 13 to account for gaze shift curvature that is independent of gaze shift direction; in the model as in the behavioral data, curvature of both gaze shift and saccade depends on head contribution.

100 • DECEMBER 2008 •

www.jn.org

Downloaded from jn.physiology.org on August 31, 2010

300

3392

E. G. FREEDMAN

Summary During repeated gaze shifts to the same target displaced along the horizontal meridian, when the eyes begin in different orbital positions, there is a reversal of the expected relationship between saccade duration and amplitude. When movements are made to a target displaced both horizontally and vertically, there is a similar reversal in the duration-amplitude relationship for the horizontal component of saccades. Under these conditions, it was possible to assess the relative roles of horizontal saccade amplitude and horizontal saccade duration on the duration of the constant amplitude vertical saccade component. The data presented here demonstrate that the duration of the vertical saccade component is poorly related to the horizontal amplitude of saccades and the linear relationship between vertical duration and horizontal amplitude can have a negative slope. However, vertical duration is positively correlated with the duration of the horizontal saccade component—independent of the horizontal component amplitude. It is not clear how most extant models of the gaze control system will be able to account for these findings. However, the Freedman (2001) J Neurophysiol • VOL

model without alterations can account for the reversal of the duration-amplitude relationship during horizontal gaze shifts, and by extending this model to two dimensions (using the cross-coupling of Becker and Ju¨rgens 1990), it is possible to simulate the results in this paper. It is now important to test the critical predictions of this scheme and determine the neural mechanisms with which it may be implemented. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The author thanks Aaron L. Cecala for assistance with data collection and, along with Drs. S. Quessy, M. Walton, and J. Quinet, for comments on an earlier version of the manuscript. Thanks also to G. Parker for technical assistance. GRANTS

This work was supported in part by National Eye Institute Grant EY-13239. REFERENCES

Bahill AT, Clark MR, Stark L. The main sequence, a tool for studying human eye movements. Math Biosci 24: 191–204, 1975. Baloh RW, Konrad HR, Sills AW, Honrubia V. The saccade velocity test. J Neurol. 25: 1071–1076, 1975. Barton EJ, Nelson JS, Gandhi NJ, Sparks DL. Effects of partial lidocaine inactivation of the paramedian pontine reticular formation on saccades of macaques. J Neurophysiol 90: 372–386, 2003. Becker W, Ju¨rgens R. Human oblique saccades: quantitative analysis of the relation between horizontal and vertical components. Vision Res 30: 893– 920, 1990. Bizzi E, Kalil RE, Morasso P, Tagliasco V. Central programming and peripheral feedback during eye-head coordination in monkeys. Bibl Ophtha. 82: 220 –232, 1972. Bizzi E, Kalil RE, Tagliasco V. Eye-head coordination in monkeys: evidence for centrally patterned organization. Science 173: 452– 454, 1971. Collewijn H. Eye- and head movements in freely moving rabbits. J Physiol 266: 471– 498, 1977. Collewijn H, Erkelens CJ, Steinman RM. Binocular co-ordination of human horizontal saccadic eye movements. J Physiol 404: 157–182, 1988a. Collewijn H, Erkelens CJ, Steinman RM. Binocular co-ordination of human vertical saccadic eye movements. J Physiol 404: 183–197, 1988b. Cullen KE, Huterer M, Braidwood DA, Sylvestre PA. Time course of vestibuloocular reflex suppression during gaze shifts. J Neurophysiol 92: 3408 –3422, 2004. Delreux V, Abeele SV, Lefevre P, Roucoux A. Influences of eye position on the control of head movement amplitude. In: Brain and Space, edited by Paillard J. Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press, 1991, p. 38 – 48. Dichgans J, Bizzi E, Morasso P, Tagliasco V. The role of vestibular and neck afferents during eye-head coordination in the monkey. Brain Res 71: 225–232, 1974. Evinger C, Kaneko CRS, Fuchs AF. Oblique saccadic eye movements of the cat. Exp Brain Res 41: 370 –379, 1981. Freedman EG. Interactions between eye and head control signals can account for movement kinematics. Biol Cybern 84: 453– 462, 2001. Freedman EG. Head-eye interactions during vertical gaze shifts made by rhesus monkeys. Exp Brain Res 167: 1–14, 2005. Freedman EG, Sparks DL. Activity of cells in the deeper layers of the superior colliculus of the rhesus monkey: evidence for a gaze displacement command. J Neurophysiol 78: 1669 –1690, 1997a. Freedman EG, Sparks DL. Eye-head coordination during head-unrestrained gaze shifts in rhesus monkeys. J Neurophysiol 77: 2328 –2348, 1997b. Freedman EG, Sparks DL. Coordination of the eyes and head: movement kinematics. Exp Brain Res 131: 22–32, 2000. Frens MA, Suzuki Y, Scherberger H, Hepp K, Henn V. The collicular code of saccade direction depends on the roll orientation of the head relative to gravity. Exp Brain Res 120: 283–290, 1998. Fuchs AF. Saccadic and smooth pursuit eye movements in the monkey. J Physiol 191: 609 – 631, 1967. Galiana HL, Guitton D. Central organization and modeling of eye-head coordination during orienting gaze shifts. In: Sensing and Controlling Motion, edited by Cohen B, Tomko D, Guedry F. New York: New York Academy of Sciences, 1992, p. 452– 471.

100 • DECEMBER 2008 •

www.jn.org

Downloaded from jn.physiology.org on August 31, 2010

To achieve gaze shifts having similar amplitudes and directions with small and large head contributions, the simulated initial positions of the eyes had to vary. It is also important to consider curvature of saccades that have the same amplitude and direction, initiated from the same positions. In Fig. 14C, position trajectories of three such simulations are shown. In each case, eye movements had vertical components of 35° and horizontal components of 38°. Horizontal head-movement amplitudes varied from 4° (black) to 25° (dark gray) to ⱕ40° (light gray). Vertical and horizontal velocity profiles for each eye movement are shown in F, and in I, vertical velocity is plotted as a function horizontal velocity for each simulated movement. As demonstrated, the model simulates both straight and curved saccade trajectories. The degree of curvature is direction-independent but correlated with the contribution of the head. These simulations are consistent with the observed behavioral results described in the preceding text. The purpose of the simulations in the preceding text is to show that the reversal (somewhat surprising in itself) need not force us to reject head restrained ideas about the saccadic burst generator or about the coupling of the horizontal and vertical components of saccadic eye movements. By incorporating the Becker and Ju¨rgens (1990) cross-coupling model into an existing gaze control model, it is possible to account for the effects of head movements on saccades during constant amplitude and direction gaze shifts. This does not preclude other formulations from also accounting for these observations. Deciding among various alternatives requires continuing to test differential predictions of various hypotheses. Development of new or extensions of existing models is necessary as new findings are uncovered. It is now incumbent on proponents of other schemes to see how their models can be altered and what assumptions are necessary to account for the data presented here. It seems unlikely that there will be a general consensus on a single hypothesis of gaze control in the near future (nor should there be). But it is critical that there is agreement on the data that models need to simulate to be considered viable alternatives. Once all alternatives achieve the same output, they can be distinguished by testing their differential predictions.

COUPLING DURING OBLIQUE GAZE SHIFTS

J Neurophysiol • VOL

Robinson DA. Models of the saccadic eye movement control system. Kybernetik 14: 71– 83, 1973. Smit AC, vanOpstal AJ, vanGisbergen JAM. Component stretching in fast and slow oblique saccades in the human. Exp Brain Res 81: 325–334, 1990. Sparks DL. Conceptual issues related to the role of the superior colliculus in the control of gaze. Curr Opin Neurobiol 9: 698 –707, 1999. Stahl J. Amplitude of human head movements associated with horizontal saccades. Exp Brain Res 126: 41–54, 1999. Stahl JS. Eye-head coordination and the variation of eye-movement accuracy with orbital eccentricity. Exp Brain Res 136: 200 –210, 2001. Stanford TR, Sparks DL. Systemic errors for saccades to remembered targets: evidence for a dissociation between saccade metrics and activity in the superior colliculus. Vision Res 34: 93–106, 1994. Tabak S, Smeets JBJ, Collewijn H. Modulation of the human vestibuloocular reflex during saccades: probing by high frequency oscillation and torque pulses to the head. J Neurophysiol 76: 3249 –3263, 1996. Tomlinson RD. Combined eye-head gaze shifts in primate. III. Contributions to the accuracy of gaze saccades. J Neurophysiol 64: 1873–1981, 1990. Tomlinson RD, Bahra PS. Combined eye-head gaze shifts in the primate. II. Interactions between saccades and the vestibuloocular reflex. J Neurophysiol 56: 1558 –1570, 1986. Tweed D. Three-dimensional model of the human eye-head saccadic system. J Neurophysiol 77: 654 – 666, 1997. van Gisbergen JAM, van Opstal AJ, Schoenmakers JJM. Experimental test of two models for the generation of oblique saccades. Exp Brain Res 57: 321–336, 1985. van Gisbergen JAM, van Opstal AJ, Tax AAM. Collicular ensemble coding of saccades based on vector summation. Neuroscience 21: 541–555, 1987. van Opstal AJ, Kappen H. A two-dimensional ensemble coding model for spatial temporal transformation for saccades in monkey superior colliculus. Network 4: 19 –38, 1993. Viirre, Tweed D, Milner K, Vilis T. A reexamination of the gain of the vestibuloocular reflex. J Neurophysiol 56: 439 – 450, 1986. Volle M, Guitton D. Human gaze shifts in which the head and eyes are not initially aligned. Exp Brain Res 94: 463– 470, 1993. Yee RD, Schiller VL, Lim V, Baloh FG, Baloh RW, Honrubia V. Velocities of vertical saccades with different eye movement recording methods. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 26: 938 –944, 1985.

100 • DECEMBER 2008 •

www.jn.org

Downloaded from jn.physiology.org on August 31, 2010

Goossens HHLM, van Opstal AJ. Human eye-head coordination in two dimensions under different sensorimotor conditions. Exp Brain Res 114: 542–560, 1997. Grossman GE, Robinson DA. Ambivalence in modelling oblique saccades. Biol Cybern 58: 13–18, 1988. Guitton D, Douglas RM, Volle M. Eye-head coordination in cats. J Neurophysiol 52: 1030 –1050, 1984. Guitton D, Mandl G. Oblique saccades of the cat: a comparison between the durations of horizontal and vertical components. Vision Res 20: 875– 881, 1980. Guitton D, Munoz DP, Gallana HL. Gaze control in the cat: Studies and modeling of the coupling between orienting eye and head movements in different behavioral tasks. J Neurophysiol 64: 509 –531, 1990. Guitton D, Volle M. Gaze control in humans: Eye-head coordination during orienting movements to targets within and beyond the oculomotor range J Neurophysiol 58: 427– 439, 1987. Judge SJ, Richmond BJ, Chu FC. Implantation of magnetic search coils for measurement of eye position: an improved method. Vision Res 20: 535–538, 1980. Ju¨rgens R, Becker W, Kornhuber HH. Natural and drug-induced variations of velocity and duration of human saccadic eye movements: evidence for a control of the neural pulse generator by local feedback. Biol Cybern 39: 87–96, 1981. King WM, Lisberger SG, Fuchs AF. Oblique saccadic eye movements of primates. J Neurophysiol 56: 769 –784, 1986. Laurutis V, Robinson D. The vestibulo-ocular reflex during human saccadic eye movements. J Physiol 373: 209 –233, 1986. Nichols MJ, Sparks DL. Component stretching during oblique stimulationevoked saccade: the role of the superior colliculus. J Neurophysiol 76: 582– 600, 1996. Pelisson D, Prablanc C. Vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) induced by passive head rotation and goal-directed saccadic eye movements do not simply add in man. Brain Res 380: 397– 400, 1986. Phillips JO, Ling L, Fuchs AF, Seibold C, Plorde JJ. Rapid horizontal gaze movement in the monkey. J Neurophysiol 73: 1632–1652, 1995. Quaia C, Optican LM. Model with distributed vectorial premotor bursters accounts for the component stretching of oblique saccades. J Neurophysiol 78: 1120 –1134, 1997.

3393